Archives of Sexual Behavior
https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2Fs10508-018-1293-5
Introduction
The Target Article by Grubbs, Perry, Wilt, and Reid (2018) addresses an important and timely topic regarding problems that individuals may experience related to pornography use. Grubbs et al. argue that there are individuals who self-identify as being addicted to pornography without having objectively dysregulated use. Grubbs et al. suggest a model of pornography problems due to moral incongruence (PPMI) that “may assist in interpreting pornography addiction literature, with a specific focus on how moral incongruence—broadly, the experience of engaging in activities that violate one’s deeply held moral values—may lead to self-perceived problems stemming from pornography use.”
The model on PPMI is worth considering. The figure that summarizes the model (see Fig. 1 in Grubbs et al., 2018) includes “distress” as the main dependent variable, differentiating three different levels: intrapersonal/psychological distress, interpersonal/relational distress, and religious/spiritual distress. The suggested processes resulting in distress include two main pathways: Pathway 1, which is referred to as “pornography problems due to dysregulation” and Pathway 2, which is termed “pornography problems due to moral incongruence.” Grubbs et al. state that Pathway 1, which reflects mechanisms of development and maintenance of an addictive use of pornography, is not the main focus of the model introduced and, instead, they liken it to other specific models (e.g., the I-PACE model) (Brand, Young, Laier, Wölfling, & Potenza, 2016b). Nevertheless, Grubbs et al. decided to include this Pathway 1 in their model, and this pathway contains several aspects of an addictive or dysregulated use of pornography. Some aspects of this pathway are connected to mechanisms of PPMI, for example, both “dysregulation” and “moral incongruence” are supposed to directly effect “self-perceived pornography-related problems,” which then result in distress.
We argue that this approach—to include a pathway on dysregulated use and to connect this pathway with the PPMI pathway—is inadequately considered by Grubbs et al. (2018). From our perspective, it would have been better to elaborate further on the connections between core elements of the two potential pathways and to more fully consider data, especially regarding other aspects not fully considered in the article, regarding, for example, motivations for abstinence and failures of self-control in such settings. Further, Grubbs et al. could place the model within contexts of current patterns of pornography viewing and other addictive behaviors within religious contexts.
Comments on Pathway 1 of the Model: Dysregulated Pornography Use
The first pathway in the model is a simplified illustration of the processes involved in the development and maintenance of what Grubbs et al. describe as addictive or dysregulated pornography use. This pathway, in its present form, includes limited individual examples of differences (e.g., impulsivity, sensation-seeking, coping deficits), as predisposing factors leading to pornography use followed by dysregulation. The figure suggests that the dysregulated behavior leads to distress, both directly and indirectly, over self-perceived pornography-related problems. However, the key factors which are associated with the dysregulation of pornography use are only mentioned incompletely and superficially by Grubbs et al. (2018). Although this pathway is not the focus of the model, it would have benefitted from including more information on the development of dysregulated use of pornography in order to better distinguish (or connect) the two pathways.
Several studies have already emphasized that there are additional individual characteristics that may promote the development of addictive or dysregulated pornography use. Prominent examples include sexual excitability and motivation (Laier & Brand, 2014; Lu, Ma, Lee, Hou, & Liao, 2014; Stark et al., 2017), social cognitions (Whang, Lee, & Chang, 2003; Yoder, Virden, & Amin, 2005), and psychopathology (Kor et al., 2014; Schiebener, Laier, & Brand, 2015; Whang et al., 2003). These characteristics may not have a direct effect on symptom severity of addictive pornography use, but the effects are moderated and/or mediated by affective and cognitive reactions to external or internal triggers and executive (inhibitory control) functions resulting in the decision to use pornography (Allen, Kannis-Dymand, & Katsikitis, 2017; Antons & Brand, 2018; Brand et al., 2016b; Schiebener et al., 2015; Snagowski & Brand, 2015). Central to addictive pornography use are cue-reactivity and craving responses (e.g., Antons & Brand, 2018; Brand, Snagowski, Laier, & Maderwald, 2016a; Gola et al., 2017; Kraus, Meshberg-Cohen, Martino, Quinones, & Potenza, 2015; Laier, Pawlikowski, Pekal, Schulte, & Brand, 2013; Snagowski, Wegmann, Pekal, Laier, & Brand, 2015; Weinstein, Zolek, Babkin, Cohen, & Lejoyeux, 2015). It has been argued that the gratification experienced when using pornography reinforces—due to conditioning processes (Banca et al., 2016; Klucken, Wehrum-Osinsky, Schweckendiek, Kruse, & Stark, 2016; Snagowski, Laier, Duka, & Brand, 2016)—the aforementioned affective responses toward pornography-related stimuli, which leads to a continued use of pornography (cf. Brand et al., 2016b). Previous studies indicate that hyperactivities of brain-reward systems, especially those including the ventral striatum, are associated with an increase in craving and other symptoms of an addictive pornography use (Brand et al., 2016a; Gola, Wordecha, Marchewka, & Sescousse, 2016; Gola et al., 2017).
In their model, Grubbs et al. (2018) potentially subsume the well-known craving concept under the term emotional dysregulation. However, craving is much more than emotional dysregulation, since it represents the emotional, motivational, and physiological responses to addiction-related stimuli (Carter et al., 2009; Carter & Tiffany, 1999; Tiffany, Carter, & Singleton, 2000) resulting in both approach and avoidance tendencies (Breiner, Stritzke, & Lang, 1999; Robinson & Berridge, 2000). The relevance of studying craving processes in relation to findings revealed by the Cyber Pornography Use Inventory-9 (CPUI-9) (Grubbs, Volk, Exline, & Pargament, 2015b) has been noted, particularly as findings relating to compulsive use of pornography (as operationalized by the “perceived compulsivity” aspect of the CPUI-9) seem sensitive to both motivation to abstain from pornography and frequency of use when attempting to abstain (Fernandez, Tee, & Fernandez, 2017).
The component of “low self-control” in the model by Grubbs et al. (2018) potentially includes or refers to reduced executive functions and inhibitory control, as inhibitors of craving responses (Bechara, 2005), which further facilitates diminished control over the use of pornography. A dysfunction of control mechanisms, such as executive functioning, when being confronted with pornographic cues and coping with stress, were found to be poorer in individuals with tendencies toward addictive pornography use (Laier & Brand, 2014; Laier, Pawlikowski, & Brand, 2014a; Laier, Pekal, & Brand, 2014b). The dysregulation of pornography use may result from an enhanced responsivity toward pornographic cues and craving as well as reduced control mechanisms promoted by individual characteristics such as high sexual motivation, loneliness, psychopathology (Brand et al., 2016b; Stark et al., 2017), and impulsivity (Antons & Brand, 2018; Romer Thomsen et al., 2018; Wéry, Deleuze, Canale, & Billieux, 2018). In the model by Grubbs et al., these complex associations are limited to one dimension that implicitly summarizes some of these aspects. However, depicting the complexity of Pathway 1 would be helpful in distinguishing more precisely between the etiology of pornography-related problems in general, whether potentially due to moral incongruence and/or addictive or dysregulated use.
Comments on Pathway 2 of the Model: Experienced Problems Related to Pornography Use Due to Moral Incongruence
Based on previous studies, Grubbs et al. (2018) illustrate the interaction of several concepts that are theoretically linked to PPMI. While the findings are based on previously published research, they suffer from assumptions about “perceived addiction” and may in part be generating a false dichotomy based on how constructs and the scale are operationalized, together with being based on a small number of potentially limited studies conducted to date.
Grubbs et al. (2018) argue that religiousness is the very first predictor of self-perceived pornography-related problems and feelings of distress in Pathway 2. Judging from the arrows, Grubbs et al. seem to suggest a (at least partial) direct effect from religiousness to self-perceived problems. In addition, Grubbs et al. included an arrow from religiousness over moral disapproval of pornography and overuse of pornography to moral incongruence and then to self-perceived pornography-related problems and feelings of distress (see Fig. 1 in Grubbs et al., 2018). This seems to indicate a partial mediation from religiousness to self-perceived pornography-related problems and feelings of distress and the mediators could be moral disapproval, pornography use, and moral incongruence. In this case, it would be very interesting to see which additional factors may contribute to using pornography since religiousness and moral values diminish its potential use. In other words: Why do people with certain moral values use pornography, although the use violates their moral values?
One observation worth mentioning is that studies included in the meta-analysis investigated largely Christian male populations. For example, in the study by Grubbs, Exline, Pargament, Hook, and Carlisle (2015a), 59% of the participants were Christians (36% Protestant or Evangelical Christians, 23% Catholic Christians), raising the question if the model is particularly designed for a certain subgroup of religious individuals. Furthermore, approximately one-third (32%) of participants in this sample were religiously unaffiliated including atheists and agnostics. This raises questions as to how Pathway 2 of the model on PPMI may be valid for non-religious individuals when religiousness is the very first predictor. There are further potential interactions between persons’ characteristics and religiousness that are possibly involved in experiencing distress related to pornography use that may relate to pornographic content. For example, in individuals with a non-heterosexual orientation (at least 10% of participants in Grubbs et al., 2015a), there may be conflicts between an individual’s religiousness and sexual orientation/preference (which might violate religious beliefs), and such conflicts may impact feelings of distress related to the use of such pornography (e.g., non-heterosexual content). Such potential interactions are important to consider when analyzing effects of religiousness on PPMI. Similarly, with current pornography frequently depicting violence toward women and having popular themes of rape and incest (Bridges, Wosnitzer, Scharrer, Sun, & Liberman, 2010; O’Neil, 2018), should such content be considered when assessing moral incongruence? Unfortunately, those motivational and pornography-content-related factors are not included explicitly in the pathway/model. We argue that the factors leading to pornography use despite incongruence with moral and/or religious values are likely more complex and nuanced than presented.
Additional factors warranting consideration may include media-specific aspects and individual characteristics. Examples of media-specific factors, which have also been summarized by Grubbs et al. (2018), are affordability, anonymity, and accessibility (the triple A engine) as suggested by Cooper (1998), and the observation that Internet pornography offers the chance to escape from reality, as suggested in the ACE-model by Young (2008). Factors leading to pornography use, even though the use violates one’s moral values, could also lie in individual characteristics, such as trait sexual motivation (Stark et al., 2017). Past experiences associated with pornography use (e.g., experienced gratification and sexual satisfaction) (cf. Brand et al., 2016b), could also increase the likelihood of using pornography (continuously), given that sexual behaviors are naturally reinforcing (cf. Georgiadis & Kringelbach, 2012).
Our main point is that more connections between the two pathways are worth considering. This is particularly important since Grubbs et al. (2018) argue that they are aiming to contribute to “interpreting pornography addiction literature.” In addition, Grubbs et al. state: “More simply, as we review below, perceived addiction (as it has been considered in prior literature) is often likely functioning as a proxy for more general views of pornography use as problematic due to feelings of moral incongruence.”
We agree with “perceived addiction” not being the ideal term and potentially highly problematic. The use of the CPUI-9 total score to define “perceived addiction” does not seem appropriate given that the three subscales incompletely assess various aspects of addiction. For example, craving is not sufficiently considered (see above), addiction is not defined by quantity/frequency measures (these may vary widely in substance-use disorders; see also discussion of quantity/frequency measures as related to CPUI-9 scores in Fernandez et al., 2017), and many other aspects relevant to addictions are not adequately considered (e.g., interference in relationships, occupation, school). Many of the CPUI-9 questions, such as those related to emotional distress and derived from measures linked to moral/religious conceptions, do not correlate well with the two more strongly correlated CPUI-9 subscales related to compulsivity and access (Grubbs et al., 2015a). For this reason, some researchers (e.g., Fernandez et al., 2017) have stated, “our findings cast doubts on the suitability of the Emotional Distress subscale as part of the CPUI-9,” particularly as it is the Emotional Distress component that consistently does not show a relationship to quantity of pornography use. Further, the inclusion of these items in a scale that defines “perceived addiction” may skew findings diminishing the contribution from perceived compulsive use and inflating the contribution of perceived moral incongruence (Grubbs et al., 2015a). While these data may provide support for the separation of these items from the others in the scale (potentially in support of the proposed model), the items focus only on feeling sick, shame, or depressed when viewing pornography. These negative feelings represent only a possible subset of negative consequences related to Internet-pornography use, and ones that are arguably linked to specific aspects of specific religious beliefs. To disentangle addictive use and PPMI, it is very important to consider not only the PPMI side, but also potential interactions between mechanisms of addictive or dysregulated use and those contributing to PPMI in order to better understand the two conditions and whether they are, indeed, separate. Grubbs et al. (2018) argue (in the section: “What about a third pathway?”) that there might be an additional pathway of problems related to pornography use, which could be the combination of experiencing “objective dysregulation” and PPMI simultaneously. We argue that a combination of both pathways may not be a third one, but possibly a mechanism underlying “both” problems with pornography use. In other words, we posit that some of addiction-related processes and motivational factors may operate across PPMI and “dysregulated use.” These similarities may exist even if the time spent viewing pornography may differ with respect to generating distress or impairment in PPMI and “dysregulated use.” In “both conditions,” pornography is used more than intended, which may result in negative consequences and distress, and pornography use is continued despite negative consequences. The psychological processes underlying such use may be similar, and these should be investigated in more detail.
Comments on Potential Connections Between the Two Pathways Instead of Suggesting a Third Pathway
Multiple important questions remain: What is the nature of PPMI in terms of underlying psychological processes? Do people who report PPMI have the feeling of diminished control over their (small or medium) use of pornography? Do they feel that it is hard to resist using pornography? Do they experience a conflict between a high motivation to use pornography on the one hand and simultaneously a feeling that pornography use is forbidden because of moral values on the other hand? It is important to better understand the nature of desire and motivation to use pornography (Brand et al., 2011; Carpenter, Janssen, Graham, Vorst, & Wicherts, 2010; Stark et al., 2015, 2017) in individuals with PPMI. The desire and motivation of pornography use, the dynamic of affective and cognitive reactions when using pornography—e.g., in terms of incentive salience theory and dual process theories of addiction (Everitt & Robbins, 2016; Robinson & Berridge, 2000)—and consequently the experienced problems to control use, could be similar in individuals with PPMI and in those with dysregulated/addictive use. In this context, an important topic is craving (see above). Do individuals reporting PPMI experience craving and the urge to use pornography in their daily lives? Are they preoccupied by using pornography? Do they frequently think about using pornography or about whether they violate their values when using pornography? Do they have negative feelings when they do not have the chance to use pornography? These questions should be addressed in future studies on PPMI in order to better understand the etiology of this phenomenon. Additionally, an interesting topic to distinguish between PPMI and an addictive use of pornography would be the pornography-use-related expectancies, as demonstrated for other types of Internet-use disorders, behavioral addictions, and substance-use disorders (Borges, Lejuez, & Felton, 2018; Taymur et al., 2016; Wegmann, Oberst, Stodt, & Brand, 2017; Xu, Turel, & Yuan, 2012). Do individuals with supposed PPMI use pornography in order to avoid negative mood or to cope with daily stress? Do they expect a strong gratification (Cooper, Delmonico, Griffin-Shelley, & Mathy, 2004) that cannot be achieved by another activity? Are there specific situations in which they feel hardly able to control their pornography use (Kraus, Rosenberg, Martino, Nich, & Potenza, 2017) even if it is a violation of moral values?
The potential connections between the two pathways would be very interesting and could inspire future research. Investigators could potentially disentangle phenomena characterizing some individuals who perceive themselves as addicted to pornography or having PPMI, respectively, despite possible differences in pornography-use quantity or frequency.
Potential connections between the two pathways could be:
Conflict between craving and moral values when being confronted with pornography-related stimuli
Conflict between value-oriented inhibitory-control processes and craving
Conflict between impulses to use pornography and moral values
Conflict between coping style and value-oriented inhibitory-control processes
Conflict between decision making with respect to short-term rewards (gratification due to pornography use) and long-term effects considering moral values
Feelings of shame and guilt after using pornography, which may result in negative mood states and potentially increase the likelihood of using pornography again in order to cope with negative mood states and feelings of distress
We argue that it is worth considering these potential interactions of processes for potential inclusion in future more comprehensive models of problematic pornography use. This could also help to disentangle specific and common mechanisms in proposed models. Future research could benefit from a more synergistic perspective rather than following two parallel lines of research that suggest orthogonality of the different types of problems related to pornography use.
Comments on Clinical Implications
Grubbs et al. (2018) argue: “Regardless of whether an individual actually experiences excessive pornography use (e.g., addiction) or PPMI, we acknowledge both clinical presentations can be associated with emotional pain, psychological suffering, and significant interpersonal consequences. It is for this reason we advance our model of PPMI as an alternative conceptualization to help illuminate what the focus of clinical attention should be.” We agree with the view that both situations (and others) deserve attention by clinicians if individuals seeking treatment experience functional impairment or distress. Specifically, as noted previously by other researchers (Fernandez et al., 2017), it is important to consider individual clinical factors including those relating to moral incongruence. However, for the clinical differentiation of an addictive use of pornography and PPMI, a better understanding of common and differential mechanisms of both phenomena is mandatory. We further argue that a combination of processes involved in multiple forms of problematic pornography use may underlie psychological distress, compulsive use, and other factors experienced by individuals and should therefore be treated individually.
Grubbs et al. (2018) state: “In short, we posit that PPMI are real problems with real psychosocial consequences, but that the etiology of those problems is distinct from a true addiction. In clinical settings, being able to differentiate between these etiological variants is likely important.” As mentioned above, we agree with the view that both aspects—PPMI and dysregulated use—deserve attention in clinical settings. We would like to emphasize this point as we believe that the views posited by Grubbs et al. should not be interpreted as minimizing the impact of pornography use on individuals and their functioning. That is, we strongly believe that a PPMI model should not be used to minimize the clinical impact of problematic pornography use in its various presentations or to draw the conclusion that pornography viewing for individuals with proposed PPMI is innocuous, over-reactive, or otherwise inconsequential. However, it is possible that the processes of the development and maintenance of both perceived compulsive/addictive use and PPMI are less distinct than suggested by Grubbs et al. and there may be parallel or possibly synergistic rather than orthogonal mechanisms explaining psychological distress. It should also be noted that distress may change relative to stages of addictions and that this model should be tested in multiple clinical populations (e.g., actively treatment-seeking versus remitted), given potentially different levels of insight relating to distress and impact. It is plausible that the etiologies of both compulsive/addictive use and moral distress share some main motivational, affective, and cognitive processes. We believe that there are open questions relating to etiology and treatment of compulsive/addictive or distressing use of pornography, and understanding factors beyond those captured by the CPUI-9 and studied to date are needed to advance research and clinical practice. In this process, consideration of multiple facets of presentation are important, including motivations for seeking treatment, impact of pornography viewing, and goals of treatment. In some cases, it likely makes sense to use Acceptance and Commitment Therapy techniques, as suggested by Grubbs et al. However, in other cases, behavior modification and other techniques of cognitive behavior therapy may be helpful if the client’s goal is to better cope with his/her desires and cravings to use pornography and his/her cognitions, inhibitory control, and pornography-related expectancies (Potenza, Sofuoglu, Carroll, & Rounsaville, 2011). Multiple aspects should be taken into account when individuals experiencing problems related to pornography use seek treatment (Kraus, Martino, & Potenza, 2016). Therefore, multiple aspects—moral incongruence and mechanisms of an addiction process, such as craving, inhibitory control, decision making—should be considered fully when examining individuals’ problems related to pornography use to provide optimalized, individualized treatment.
Notes
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. Dr. Brand has received (to University of Duisburg-Essen) grants from the German Research Foundation (DFG), the German Federal Ministry for Research and Education, the German Federal Ministry for Health, and the European Union. Dr. Brand has performed grant reviews for several agencies; has edited journal sections and articles; has given academic lectures in clinical or scientific venues; and has generated books or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts. Dr. Potenza has consulted for and advised Rivermend Health, Opiant/Lakelight Therapeutics, and Jazz Pharmaceuticals; received research support (to Yale) from the Mohegan Sun Casino and the National Center for Responsible Gaming; consulted for or advised legal and gambling entities on issues related to impulse control and addictive behaviors; provided clinical care related to impulse control and addictive behaviors; performed grant reviews; edited journals/journal sections; given academic lectures in grand rounds, CME events, and other clinical/scientific venues; and generated books or chapters for publishers of mental health texts.
References
- Allen, A., Kannis-Dymand, L., & Katsikitis, M. (2017). Problematic internet pornography use: The role of craving, desire thinking, and metacognition. Addictive Behaviors, 70, 65–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.02.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Antons, S., & Brand, M. (2018). Trait and state impulsivity in males with tendency towards Internet-pornography-use disorder. Addictive Behaviors, 79, 171–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.12.029.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Banca, P., Morris, L. S., Mitchell, S., Harrison, N. A., Potenza, M. N., & Voon, V. (2016). Novelty, conditioning and attentional bias to sexual rewards. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 72, 91–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.10.017.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1458–1463. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1584.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Borges, A. M., Lejuez, C. W., & Felton, J. W. (2018). Positive alcohol use expectancies moderate the association between anxiety sensitivity and alcohol use across adolescence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 187, 179–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.02.029.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Brand, M., Laier, C., Pawlikowski, M., Schächtle, U., Schöler, T., & Altstötter-Gleich, C. (2011). Watching pornographic pictures on the Internet: Role of sexual arousal ratings and psychological–psychiatric symptoms for using Internet sex sites excessively. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 371–377. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brand, M., Snagowski, J., Laier, C., & Maderwald, S. (2016a). Ventral striatum activity when watching preferred pornographic pictures is correlated with symptoms of Internet pornography addiction. Neuroimage, 129, 224–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.033.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Brand, M., Young, K. S., Laier, C., Wölfling, K., & Potenza, M. N. (2016b). Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders: An Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 71, 252–266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Breiner, M. J., Stritzke, W. G., & Lang, A. R. (1999). Approaching avoidance. A step essential to the understanding of craving. Alcohol Research & Health, 23, 197–206. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018783329341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bridges, A. J., Wosnitzer, R., Scharrer, E., Sun, C., & Liberman, R. (2010). Aggression and sexual behavior in best-selling pornography videos: A content analysis update. Violence Against Women, 16, 1065–1085. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801210382866.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Carpenter, D. L., Janssen, E., Graham, C. A., Vorst, H., & Wicherts, J. (2010). The sexual inhibition/sexual excitation scales-short form SIS/SES-SF. In T. D. Fisher, C. M. Davis, W. L. Yarber, & S. L. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of sexuality-related measures (Vol. 3, pp. 236–239). Abingdon, GB: Routledge.Google Scholar
- Carter, B. L., Lam, C. Y., Robinson, J. D., Paris, M. M., Waters, A. J., Wetter, D. W., & Cinciripini, P. M. (2009). Generalized craving, self-report of arousal, and cue reactivity after brief abstinence. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 11, 823–826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Carter, B. L., & Tiffany, S. T. (1999). Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction, 94, 327–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Cooper, A. (1998). Sexuality and the Internet: Surfing into the new millennium. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 1, 181–187. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.1998.1.187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Cooper, A., Delmonico, D., Griffin-Shelley, E., & Mathy, R. (2004). Online sexual activity: An examination of potentially problematic behaviors. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 11, 129–143. https://doi.org/10.1080/10720160490882642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2016). Drug addiction: Updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 23–50. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033457.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Fernandez, D. P., Tee, E. Y. J., & Fernandez, E. F. (2017). Do cyber pornography use inventory-9 scores reflect actual compulsivity in Internet pornography use? Exploring the role of abstinence effort. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 24, 156–179. https://doi.org/10.1080/10720162.2017.1344166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Georgiadis, J. R., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2012). The human sexual response cycle: Brain imaging evidence linking sex to other pleasures. Progress in Neurobiology, 98, 49–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gola, M., Wordecha, M., Marchewka, A., & Sescousse, G. (2016). Visual sexual stimuli—Cue or reward? A perspective for interpreting brain imaging findings on human sexual behaviors. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 16, 402. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gola, M., Wordecha, M., Sescousse, G., Lew-Starowicz, M., Kossowski, B., Wypych, M., & Marchewka, A. (2017). Can pornography be addictive? An fMRI study of men seeking treatment for problematic pornography use. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42, 2021–2031. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2017.78.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Grubbs, J. B., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., Hook, J. N., & Carlisle, R. D. (2015a). Transgression as addiction: Religiosity and moral disapproval as predictors of perceived addiction to pornography. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44, 125–136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0257-z.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Grubbs, J. B., Perry, S. L., Wilt, J. A., & Reid, R. C. (2018). Pornography problems due to moral incongruence: An integrative model with a systematic review and meta-analysis. Archives of Sexual Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1248-x.
- Grubbs, J. B., Volk, F., Exline, J. J., & Pargament, K. I. (2015b). Internet pornography use: Perceived addiction, psychological distress, and the validation of a brief measure. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 41, 83–106. https://doi.org/10.1080/0092623X.2013.842192.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Klucken, T., Wehrum-Osinsky, S., Schweckendiek, J., Kruse, O., & Stark, R. (2016). Altered appetitive conditioning and neural connectivity in subjects with compulsive sexual behavior. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 13, 627–636. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsxm.2016.01.013.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kor, A., Zilcha-Mano, S., Fogel, Y. A., Mikulincer, M., Reid, R. C., & Potenza, M. N. (2014). Psychometric development of the Problematic Pornography Use Scale. Addictive Behaviors, 39, 861–868. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.01.027.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kraus, S. W., Martino, S., & Potenza, M. N. (2016). Clinical characteristics of men interested in seeking treatment for use of pornography. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5, 169–178. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.036.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Kraus, S. W., Meshberg-Cohen, S., Martino, S., Quinones, L. J., & Potenza, M. N. (2015). Treatment of compulsive pornography use with naltrexone: A case report. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(12), 1260–1261. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15060843.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kraus, S. W., Rosenberg, H., Martino, S., Nich, C., & Potenza, M. N. (2017). The development and initial evaluation of the Pornography-Use Avoidance Self-Efficacy Scale. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6, 354–363. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.057.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Laier, C., & Brand, M. (2014). Empirical evidence and theoretical considerations on factors contributing to cybersex addiction from a cognitive-behavioral view. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 21, 305–321. https://doi.org/10.1080/10720162.2014.970722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Laier, C., Pawlikowski, M., & Brand, M. (2014a). Sexual picture processing interferes with decision-making under ambiguity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 473–482. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0119-8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Laier, C., Pawlikowski, M., Pekal, J., Schulte, F. P., & Brand, M. (2013). Cybersex addiction: Experienced sexual arousal when watching pornography and not real-life sexual contacts makes the difference. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2(2), 100–107. https://doi.org/10.1556/jba.2.2013.002.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Laier, C., Pekal, J., & Brand, M. (2014b). Cybersex addiction in heterosexual female users of Internet pornography can be explained by gratification hypothesis. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17, 505–511. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2013.0396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lu, H., Ma, L., Lee, T., Hou, H., & Liao, H. (2014). The link of sexual sensation seeking to acceptance of cybersex, multiple sexual partners, and one-night stands among Taiwanese college students. Journal of Nursing Research, 22, 208–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- O’Neil, L. (2018). Incest is the fastest growing trend in porn. Wait, what? Retrieved from https://www.esquire.com/lifestyle/sex/a18194469/incest-porn-trend/.
- Potenza, M. N., Sofuoglu, M., Carroll, K. M., & Rounsaville, B. J. (2011). Neuroscience of behavioral and pharmacological treatments for addictions. Neuron, 69, 695–712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.009.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2000). The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: An incentive-sensitization view. Addiction, 95, S91–117. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.95.8s2.19.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Romer Thomsen, K., Callesen, M. B., Hesse, M., Kvamme, T. L., Pedersen, M. M., Pedersen, M. U., & Voon, V. (2018). Impulsivity traits and addiction-related behaviors in youth. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7, 317–330. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Schiebener, J., Laier, C., & Brand, M. (2015). Getting stuck with pornography? Overuse or neglect of cybersex cues in a multitasking situation is related to symptoms of cybersex addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4(1), 14–21. https://doi.org/10.1556/jba.4.2015.1.5.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Snagowski, J., & Brand, M. (2015). Symptoms of cybersex addiction can be linked to both approaching and avoiding pornographic stimuli: Results from an analog sample of regular cybersex users. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 653. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00653.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Snagowski, J., Laier, C., Duka, T., & Brand, M. (2016). Subjective craving for pornography and associative learning predict tendencies towards cybersex addiction in a sample of regular cybersex users. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 23, 342–360. https://doi.org/10.1080/10720162.2016.1151390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Snagowski, J., Wegmann, E., Pekal, J., Laier, C., & Brand, M. (2015). Implicit associations in cybersex addiction: Adaption of an Implicit Association Test with pornographic pictures. Addictive Behaviors, 49, 7–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.05.009.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Stark, R., Kagerer, S., Walter, B., Vaitl, D., Klucken, T., & Wehrum-Osinsky, S. (2015). Trait sexual motivation questionnaire: Concept and validation. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 12, 1080–1091. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsm.12843.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Stark, R., Kruse, O., Wehrum-Osinsky, S., Snagowski, J., Brand, M., Walter, B., & Klucken, T. (2017). Predictors for (problematic) use of Internet sexually explicit material: Role of trait sexual motivation and implicit approach tendencies towards sexual explicit material. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 24, 180–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Taymur, I., Budak, E., Demirci, H., Akdağ, H. A., Güngör, B. B., & Özdel, K. (2016). A study of the relationship between internet addiction, psychopathology and dysfunctional beliefs. Computers in Human Behavior, 61, 532–536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Tiffany, S. T., Carter, B. L., & Singleton, E. G. (2000). Challenges in the manipulation, assessment and interpretation of craving relevant variables. Addiction, 95, 177–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wegmann, E., Oberst, U., Stodt, B., & Brand, M. (2017). Online-specific fear of missing out and Internet-use expectancies contribute to symptoms of Internet-communication disorder. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 5, 33–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Weinstein, A. M., Zolek, R., Babkin, A., Cohen, K., & Lejoyeux, M. (2015). Factors predicting cybersex use and difficulties in forming intimate relationships among male and female users of cybersex. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 54. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00054.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Wéry, A., Deleuze, J., Canale, N., & Billieux, J. (2018). Emotionally laden impulsivity interacts with affect in predicting addictive use of online sexual activity in men. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 80, 192–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.10.004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Whang, L. S., Lee, S., & Chang, G. (2003). Internet over-users’ psychological profiles: A behavior sampling analysis on internet addiction. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 6, 143–150. https://doi.org/10.1089/109493103321640338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Xu, Z. C., Turel, O., & Yuan, Y. F. (2012). Online game addiction among adolescents: Motivation and prevention factors. European Journal of Information Systems, 21, 321–340. https://doi.org/10.1057/ejis.2011.56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Yoder, V. C., Virden, T. B., & Amin, K. (2005). Internet pornography and loneliness: An association? Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 12, 19–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/10720160590933653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Young, K. S. (2008). Internet sex addiction: Risk factors, stages of development, and treatment. American Behavioral Scientist, 52, 21–37. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764208321339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar