Sexual Coercion by Women: The Influence of Pornography and Narcissistic and Histrionic Personality Disorder Traits (2019)

COMMENTS: It’s not just the guys who are affected by porn use. A new study on women correlates porn use & porn addiction with sexual coercion, such as trying get a partner drunk or taking advantage of an intoxicated individual, Persistent kissing and touching, Emotional manipulation/deception to have sex, etc.

Note: the phrase “effort to engage” indicates pornography addiction.

——————————————————————————————————————————————–

Arch Sex Behav. 2019 Oct 7. doi: 10.1007/s10508-019-01538-4.

Hughes A1, Brewer G2, Khan R3.

Abstract

Largely overlooked in the literature, this study investigated factors influencing women’s use of sexual coercion. Specifically, pornography use and personality disorder traits linked with poor impulse control, emotional regulation, and superior sense of sexual desirability were considered. Women (N = 142) aged 16-53 years (M = 24.23, SD = 7.06) were recruited from community and student populations. Participants completed the Narcissistic and Histrionic subscales of the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire-4, in addition to the Cyber-Pornography Use Inventory to explore the influence of their pornography use (interest, efforts to engage with pornography, and compulsivity) on their use of sexual coercion. This was measured using four subscales of the Postrefusal Sexual Persistence Scale: nonverbal sexual arousal, emotional manipulation and deception, exploitation of the intoxicated, and use of physical force or threats. Multiple regression analyses revealed that pornography use, narcissistic traits, and histrionic traits significantly predicted the use of nonverbal sexual arousal, emotional manipulation and deception, and exploitation of the intoxicated. Effort to engage with pornography was a significant individual predictor of nonverbal sexual arousal and emotional manipulation and deception, while histrionic traits were a significant individual predictor of exploitation of the intoxicated. Findings were discussed in relation to existing sexual coercion literature and potential future research.

KEYWORDS: Female perpetration; Histrionic personality traits; Narcissistic personality traits; Sexually explicit material

PMID: 31591667

DOI: 10.1007/s10508-019-01538-4

Introduction

Sexual aggression research has historically focused on male perpetration and female victimization. This approach most likely reflects the global pervasiveness of men’s sexual violence and perceptions of women as sexually passive (Denov, ; Krahé & Berger, ). However, females also sexually aggress against unwilling partners (Erulkar, ; Hines, ) and researchers have increasingly acknowledged nuances in how this might be expressed (e.g., by harassment, abuse, and coercion) (Grayston & De Luca, ; Ménard, Hall, Phung, Ghebrial, & Martin, ). Despite this, and the negative physical and psychological consequences experienced by male victims (Visser, Smith, Rissel, Richters, & Grulich, ), a dominant gendered perspective has resulted in a relative paucity of information on factors that may explain female sexual aggression (Campbell & Kohut, ; Denov, ). This area is worthy of investigation as pathways to sexual aggression differ for men and women (Krahé & Berger, ), and factors associated with sexual coercion by men may not be generalizable to female perpetrators. Indeed, Schatzel-Murphy, Harris, Knight, and Milburn () found that while men and women’s sexually coercive behavior may be similar, factors symptomatic of its use might be different, with sexual compulsivity (i.e., difficulty controlling sexual urges) shown to be a dynamic influence for females. Our study, therefore, aimed to investigate factors associated with sexual compulsivity in women that might explain their use of sexually coercive behavior. Specifically, the influence of three elements of pornography use (interest, efforts to engage with pornography, and compulsivity) and narcissistic and histrionic personality traits was explored due to associations in the literature with coercive sexual tactics to obtain intimate relations.

Sexual coercion lies on the sexual aggression continuum and is defined as “the act of using pressure, alcohol or drugs, or force to have sexual contact with someone against his or her will” (Struckman-Johnson, Struckman-Johnson, & Anderson, , p.76). Sexual coercion may include a range of behaviors that can be separated into four categories of increasing exploitation: (1) sexual arousal (e.g., persistent kissing and touching), (2) emotional manipulation (e.g., blackmail, questioning, or using authority), (3) alcohol and drug intoxication (e.g., purposefully getting a person drunk or taking advantage while intoxicated), and (4) physical force or threats (e.g., using physical harm). As a large body of research has established that men are more likely than women to perpetrate sexual coercion (see Krahé et al., ), this has overshadowed evidence that a proportion of women also report using a range of sexually coercive behavior (e.g., Hoffmann & Verona, ; Krahé, Waizenhöfer, & Möller, ; Ménard et al., ; Muñoz, Khan, & Cordwell, ; Russell & Oswald, , ; Struckman-Johnson et al., ). While single studies have found female perpetration rates as high as 26% (compared to 43% for males) (see Struckman-Johnson et al., ), in an overview of the literature, Hines () estimated rates between 10 and 20% for verbal sexual coercion, and 1 and 3% for physically forced sexual intercourse.

Due to higher rates of male perpetration, it is perhaps not surprising that fewer studies have focused on correlates of women’s sexually coercive behavior. Studies have reported that influential factors for women include peer pressure to have sex (e.g., Krahé et al., ), sexual compulsivity (Schatzel-Murphy et al., ), antagonistic attitudes toward sexual relationships (e.g., Anderson, ; Christopher, Madura, & Weaver, ; Yost & Zurbriggen, ), and sexual victimization experiences (e.g., Anderson, ; Krahé et al., ; Russell & Oswald, ). Further studies have documented the influence of a hostile personality with a dominant interpersonal style (Ménard et al., ) a manipulative, game-playing approach to forming intimate relations (Russell & Oswald, , ), and pornography use (e.g., Kernsmith & Kernsmith, ) thereby providing the rationale for this study.

Women’s Use of Pornography

Pornography refers to sexually explicit material developed and consumed to stimulate sexual arousal, available in versatile forms (e.g., photographs and videos) and often accessed online (Campbell & Kohut, ). Research has historically focused on the manner in which exposure to pornographic material influences men’s sexual attitudes and conduct. For example, it is argued that men’s use of pornography is related to sexual objectification of partners (Tylka, & Kroon Van Diest, ) and sexually coercive behavior (Stanley et al., ). Compulsive consumption of pornographic material, in particular, may be closely related to men’s sexually aggressive behavior (Gonsalves, Hodges, & Scalora, ). Research indicates that women also engage with pornography, although to a lesser extent than men (Ashton, McDonald, & Kirkman, ; Rissel, Richters, de Visser, McKee, Yeung, & Caruana, ). Due to disparities in methodology, estimates of women’s pornography use vary significantly across studies, ranging from 1 to 88% depending on the sample and operational definition of pornography (Campbell & Kohut, ). In a review of their annual statistics, Pornhub, a large Internet pornography website, reported that just over a quarter of their visitors were women and that their top trending1 search throughout 2017 was “porn for women,” representing a 1400% increase (Pornhub Insights, ). While some studies report that females were more likely to use pornography with a partner (e.g., Ševčíková & Daneback, ), other studies have found that their pornography use was more likely and more frequent when alone than with a partner (Fisher, Kohut, & Campbell, ).

Consistent with studies of men’s pornography consumption, research has found women’s use of pornography to be associated with attitudes toward sex, sexual conduct, and sexual activities (e.g., number of sexual partners) (Wright, Bae, & Funk, ). This is supported further by a recent meta-analysis that found, similar to men, women’s pornography use was associated with sexual aggression, both verbally (i.e., “verbally coercive but not physically threatening communication to obtain sex, and sexual harassment”) and physically (i.e., “use or threat of physical force to obtain sex”) (Wright, Tokunaga, & Kraus, , p.191). The small number of studies in this area has meant the extent to which women’s use of pornography influences their sexually aggressive behavior remains unclear. In one such study, it was found that pornography use predicted all forms of sexual aggression in women (i.e., extortion, deceit, obligation, and emotional manipulation) except for physical violence and intimidation (Kernsmith & Kernsmith, ). The dearth of literature available indicates there is scope to investigate this further, thus we consider three elements of women’s pornography use, that is (1) interest in pornography, (2) efforts to engage with pornography, in additional to (3) pornography compulsivity, which is largely overlooked despite its association with men’s sexual aggression (e.g., Gonsalves et al., ).

Narcissistic and Histrionic Personality Disorder Traits

Personality traits may also influence the likelihood of sexually aggressive behavior in women (Krahé et al., ; Russell, Doan, & King, ). Characteristics of the dramatic, emotional, and erratic Cluster B personality disorders (associated with poor impulse control, emotional regulation, and anger) may be particularly influential on sexual aggression (Mouilso & Calhoun, ). For example, narcissistic personality disorder (NPD), found in both men (7.7%) and women (4.8%) and overall in 6.2% of the general population (Stinson et al., ), is characterized by a grandiose sense of the self, entitlement, and low empathy for others (Emmons, ). In men, narcissistic personality traits are positively associated with rape supportive beliefs and negatively associated with empathy for rape victims (Bushman, Bonacci, van Dijk, & Baumeister, ), while NPD is related to perpetration of sexual aggression (Mouilso & Calhoun, ). Women with higher levels of narcissism display more negative relationship communication (Lamkin, Lavner, & Shaffer, ) and are more likely to engage in sexual harassment (Zeigler-Hill, Besser, Morag, & Campbell, ). Pertinently, narcissism is associated with women’s perpetration of sexual coercion (Kjellgren, Priebe, Svedin, Mossige, & Långström, ; Logan, ), with the entitlement/exploitativeness dimension found to be most influential (Blinkhorn, Lyons, & Almond, ; Ryan, Weikel, & Sprechini, ). Additionally, females high in narcissism were found to be just as likely as their male counterparts to react with persistence and sexually coercive tactics after being denied during a sexual advance (Blinkhorn et al., ). In part, this behavior may reflect the tendency for narcissistic individuals to engage in sex in order to fulfill their need for self-affirmation (Gewirtz-Meydan, ).

Found in 1–3% of general population (Torgersen et al., ) and reported twice more in women than in men (Torgersen, Kringlen, & Cramer, ), traits associated with histrionic personality disorder (HPD) are far less explored than NPD in relation to sexual coercion. This is somewhat surprising as defining characteristics of HPD include excessively emotional, impulsive, attention seeking behavior, and inappropriate or competitive sexual conduct (APA, ; Dorfman, ; Stone, ). Emotionally manipulative and intolerant of delayed gratification (Bornstein & Malka, ; Stone, ), women with HPD demand confirmation and attention from intimate partners (AlaviHejazi, Fatehizade, Bahrami, & Etemadi, ). A study that compared women with HPD to a matched control group without personality disorders found they were more likely to have been sexually unfaithful and report greater sexual preoccupation and sexual boredom with lower levels of sexual assertiveness and relationship satisfaction (Apt & Hurlbert, ). Furthermore, Apt and Hurlbert considered that HPD behavioral traits were indicative of sexual narcissism, while Widiger and Trull () noted that HPD and NPD traits were likely to co-occur. The dominant, manipulative, and sexually compulsive behavioral traits found in these studies of women with NPD and HPD are pertinent as they align with extant studies reporting factors underpinning women’s perpetration of sexual coercion (e.g., Russell & Oswald, , ; Schatzel-Murphy et al., ) and pornography use (e.g., Wright et al., , ). Hence, additional research is necessary to examine the influence of both HPD and NPD traits and pornography use on women’s use of sexual aggression.

Research Aims

This study investigated the influence of pornography use and narcissistic and histrionic personality traits on four types of sexual coercion. In line with past research, we predicted that pornography use (e.g., Kernsmith & Kernsmith, ; Wright et al., ) and narcissistic and histrionic personality traits (e.g., Apt & Hurlbert, ; Blinkhorn et al., ; Kjellgren et al., ; Logan, ; Ryan et al., ) would be significantly associated with greater incidence of three types of sexual coercion (i.e., nonverbal sexual arousal, emotional manipulation and deception, and exploitation of the intoxicated). We also predicted that pornography use and personality traits would not be associated with the use of a fourth type of sexual coercion (i.e., physical force or threats) as this has not been reported in the previous research.

Method

Participants and Procedure

A total of 142 women, aged 16–53 years (M = 24.23, SD = 7.06), participated in this study. Women were typically in a long-term relationship, of at least 6 months duration (n = 53.5%). The remaining participants were single or divorced (n = 24.7%), in a short-term relationship (n = 11.3%), or married (n = 10.6%). Most participants were heterosexual (n = 85.2%), with a smaller number of bisexual (n = 11.3%) and homosexual (n = 3.5%) women recruited. Just under a half (n = 43%) of these women reported that they currently used pornography. No other demographic data were collected. Two modes of opportunity sampling were used to collect information from a diverse sample of women aged over 16 years, in a student and community population, with no known offending history. Participants volunteered to complete either a paper or online questionnaire, estimated to take 15 min. Remuneration was not offered for taking part in this study.

Participants were recruited via undergraduate and postgraduate classes plus recreational spaces within a large university in England, as well as in the local community, inside shopping centers (n = 37). The first author distributed questionnaire booklets to potential participants placed inside a self-addressed envelope, to ensure confidential and anonymous return. To obtain informed consent, potential participants were verbally informed of the anonymous and voluntary nature of the questionnaire, which was reiterated on a briefing sheet attached to the questionnaire. This briefing sheet also made clear that questionnaires should be completed alone and that return of questionnaires indicated consent for information to be used. On campus, participants were told they could place completed questionnaires in envelopes to return either to the researcher by hand or to a secure drop-in box in a student resource room. Participants were also recruited via snowballing methods using social media postings on Facebook and Twitter (n = 108). These posts detailed the study’s aims and invited females to participate by clicking on a hyperlink that redirected them to view the questionnaire online, so it could be completed securely and remotely.

Measures

Sexual Coercion: Postrefusal Sexual Persistence Scale (PSP Scale, Struckman-Johnson et al., )

The PSP Scale is a 19-item measure of postrefusal sexual persistence, defined as pursuing sexual contact with a partner after they initially refused. The scale is separated into four sections reflecting different levels of sexual exploitation: (1) nonverbal sexual arousal tactics (three items, e.g., “Persistent kissing and touching”); (2) emotional manipulation and deception strategies (eight items, e.g., “Threatening to break up”); (3) exploitation of the intoxicated (two items, e.g., “Purposely getting them drunk”), and (4) use of physical force or threats (six items, e.g., “Tying them up”). Items were scored 1 (yes) or 0 (no), with higher scores indicating greater use of sexual coercion. The internal reliability for each subscale has been mixed in previous studies (e.g., Khan, Brewer, Kim, & Centifanti, ), which was reflected in this study: nonverbal sexual arousal (α = .81); emotional manipulation and deception (α = .39); exploitation of the intoxicated (α = .38); and the use of physical force or threats (α = .00).

Pornography Use: Cyber-Pornography Use Inventory (CPUI, Grubbs, Sessoms, Wheeler, & Volk, )

Three CPUI subscales were employed: interest (two items, i.e., “I have some pornographic sites bookmarked” and “I spend more than 5 h per week using pornography”), efforts to engage with pornography (five items, e.g., “I have rearranged my schedule so that I would be able to view pornography online without being disturbed” and “I have refused to go out with friends or attend certain social functions to have the opportunity to view pornography”), and compulsivity (11 items, e.g., “When I am unable to access pornography online, I feel anxious, angry, or disappointed” and “I feel unable to stop my use of pornography”). One final item “I believe I am addicted to Internet pornography” was not included due to the controversial nature of the terms “sexual addiction” and “pornography addiction” (Schneider, ). On the interest and effort subscales, participants indicated responses as “true” (scored 2) or “false” (scored 1), while on the compulsivity subscale, responses were recorded on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree), with higher scores indicating a greater degree of pornography interest, effort, and compulsion. Reliabilities were: interest α = .40; effort α = .58; and compulsivity α = .75.

Narcissistic and Histrionic Personality Disorder Traits: Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire, 4th Edition (PDQ-4: Hyler, )

Items in the PDQ-4 Narcissistic and Histrionic subscales are based on the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for Axis II disorders and have been used in comparable studies to explore personality disorder traits and use of sexual coercion in females (e.g., Khan et al., ; Muñoz et al., ). Scores on the Narcissistic subscale (nine items, e.g., “Some people think that I take advantage of others”) and Histrionic subscale (eight items, e.g., “I am sexier than most”) were obtained by summing “false” (scored 0) or “true” (scored 1) responses, with a higher score indicating a greater level of traits associated with narcissistic and histrionic personality. Reliabilities were: narcissistic α = .63 and histrionic α = .47.

Results

Nonverbal sexual coercion (35.2%) was the most commonly reported form of sexual coercion, followed by the use of emotional manipulation and deception (15.5%), and exploitation of the intoxicated (4.9%). As only one woman reported using physical force or threats, this subscale was not included in subsequent analyses. Correlation analyses (Table 1) demonstrated positive associations between the nonverbal sexual arousal form of sexual coercion, both pornography interest and effort, and HPD traits. Both the use of emotional manipulation and deception to coerce a partner and exploitation of the intoxicated were positively correlated with both pornography effort and compulsivity, and HPD traits. Additional correlations were identified between variables and between forms of sexual coercive behavior.

Table 1

Correlations between pornography interest, effort, and compulsivity, narcissistic and histrionic personality disorder traits, and sexual coercion

POI

POE

POC

NPD

HPD

NVA

EMD

EXI

POI

POE

.36**

POC

.13

.38**

NPD

.01

.15

−.05

HPD

.04

.28**

.18*

.45**

NVA

.17*

.27**

.06

.09

.22**

EMD

.14

.38**

.24**

.12

.25**

.34**

EXI

.11

.22**

.20*

−.02

.29**

.33**

.27**

M

2.04

5.29

17.01

1.75

2.49

.58

.21

.06

SD

.18

.70

5.39

1.72

1.61

.93

.54

.26

Range

2–4

5–10

11–77

0–9

0–8

0–3

0–8

0–2

POI pornography interest, POE pornography effort, POC pornography compulsivity, NPD narcissistic personality disorder traits, HPD histrionic personality disorder traits, NVA nonverbal sexual arousal, EMD emotional manipulation and deception, EXI exploitation of the intoxicated

*p < .05, **p < .01

A series of multiple linear regressions were conducted to determine whether pornography interest, efforts, and compulsivity as well as NPD and HPD traits were predictors of sexual coercion (nonverbal sexual arousal, emotional manipulation and deception, and exploitation of the intoxicated) (see Table 2). The regression model was a significant predictor of nonverbal sexual arousal, F(5, 136) = 3.28, p = .008, explaining 10.8% of the sexual coercion variance (R2 = .11, Adj R2 = .08). Pornography effort was the only individual predictor significantly associated with this form of sexual coercion (Β = .22, t = 2.29, p = .024). A second regression revealed that the model was a significant predictor of emotional manipulation and deception, F(5, 136) = 5.83, p < .001, explaining 17.6%  % of the sexual coercion variance (R2 = .18, Adj R2 = .15). Pornography effort is the only significant individual predictor of emotional manipulation and deception (Β = .29, t = 3.14, p = .002). Finally, a third regression indicated that the model was a significant predictor of exploitation of the intoxicated, F(5,136) = 4.47, p = .001, explaining 14.1% of the sexual coercion variance (R2 = .14, Adj R2 = .11). HPD traits were the only significant individual predictor (Β = .32, t = 3.45, p = .001).

Table 2

Multiple linear regression results for pornography interest, effort, and compulsivity, narcissistic and histrionic disorder personality traits, and sexual coercion

Coercive behavior

ANOVA

R 2

Adj R2

Individual predictor

Β

t

p

Nonverbal sexual arousal

F(5, 136) = 3.28, p = .008

.11

.08

Interest

.09

1.05

.295

Effort

.22

2.29

.024

Compulsivity

− .07

− .81

.421

Narcissistic

− .03

− .29

.776

Histrionic

.18

1.87

.063

Emotional manipulation and deception

F(5, 136) = 5.83, p < .001

.18

.15

Interest

.01

.17

.869

Effort

.29

3.14

.002

Compulsivity

.11

1.24

.217

Narcissistic

.01

.14

.888

Histrionic

.15

1.61

.111

Exploitation of the intoxicated

F(5, 136) = 4.47, p = .001

.14

.11

Interest

.05

.53

.596

Effort

.11

1.15

.253

Compulsivity

.08

.96

.337

Narcissistic

− .17

− 1.93

.056

Histrionic

.32

3.45

.001

Discussion

Confirming expectations, pornography effort was associated with women’s use of the nonverbal sexual arousal and emotional manipulation and deception forms of sexual coercion. This finding is broadly consistent with the previous research that links women’s pornography use with a range of sexual coercive behaviors, such as harassment, verbal coercion, emotional manipulation, and deceit (Kernsmith & Kernsmith, ; Wright et al., ), though additional research is required to consider why pornography interest and compulsivity were not associated with sexually coercive behavior. As there is little in terms of comparable research, explanations for these findings are proposed with caution. For example, as the previous research with male participants found compulsive pornography use to be related to the use of sexual coercion (e.g., Gonsalves et al., ), this disparity may reflect a sex difference. However, the alpha coefficients for the sexual compulsivity measures used in their study were low, confounding efforts to compare findings. As this area merits further exploration, it would be prudent for future studies to explore different elements of pornography use and sex differences further.

Our study also found that HPD traits were significantly associated with exploitation of the intoxicated, which the literature indicates may reflect excessive emotionality, demands for attention, and use of provocative behavior to manipulate others (e.g., AlaviHejazi et al., ; Bornstein & Malka, ; Dorfman, ; Stone, ). Indeed, women may be more likely to coerce a partner when feeling rejected (Wright, Norton, & Matusek, ). Unlike men (who are reportedly more likely than women to be motivated by power), sexually coercive women are reported to be motivated by affiliation–intimacy (Zurbriggen, ), which may be exaggerated in women with HPD traits who display increased sexual preoccupation (Apt & Hurlbert, ). The use of coercive behavior to sexually exploit the intoxicated could reflect the low levels of sexual assertiveness reported in women with HPD (see Apt & Hurlbert, ), thereby inhibiting the use of other forms of sexual coercion that require some degree of force. We did not observe the expected influence of NPD traits on sexual coercion. This was predicted due to previously reported associations between narcissism, sexual harassment (Zeigler-Hill et al., ), and coercion (Blinkhorn et al., ). This finding could also be indicative of similarities between NPD and HPD traits (as noted by Apt & Hurlbert, ; Widiger & Trull, ); thus, it would be advantageous for future investigations to explore this more explicitly.

As extant research is sparse and findings mixed, we did not make predictions on the use of physical force or threats to coerce a partner, and ultimately, as only one participant reported this, this subscale was excluded from analysis. Studies that do not include pornography use as a potential factor for sexual coercion report that women are less likely to use physical force or threats than they are to use other sexually coercive behavior, such as verbal pressure (Krahé et al., ), possibly indicative of greater caution or fear of retaliation. Indeed, female perpetrators of sexual coercion experience more negative reactions and resistance by victims than male perpetrators (O’Sullivan, Byers, & Finkelman, ). Yet, to complicate this further, studies that do examine the influence of pornography use on sexual coercion report contrary findings. For example, a meta-analysis of 22 studies found that women’s pornography use predicted all forms of sexual coercion, including physical force and threats (e.g., Wright et al., ), while another study found, to the contrary, that women’s pornography use was not associated with physical intimidation and force (e.g., Kernsmith & Kernsmith, ). Future research could investigate these elements collectively to consider whether use of pornography influences women to employ physical force or threats only when other forms of sexual coercion fail, or if there are specific factors that explain the use of physical force and threatening behavior.

Limitations and Further Research Directions

Despite efforts to recruit more participants, this study was limited by its use of a small, non-probabilistic sample; thus, generalizability is limited. As noted in other studies, the use of self-report questionnaire measures to investigate the sensitive topic of sexual coercion perpetration (e.g., Gonsalves et al., ) and personality disorder traits (Hoffmann & Verona, ; Khan et al., ; Muñoz et al., ) may have resulted in social desirability or bias recall. Further, the Cronbach’s alphas for some subscales were low. In part, this reflects the nature of the measure. (The exploitation of the intoxicated and pornography interest subscales contained two items each.) More extensive, detailed measures are recommended for future research. In particular, it was an oversight to overlook the potential influence of different types of pornographic materials, as women are exposed to a range of sexually explicit materials, including violent versus nonviolent pornography (Mattebo, Tyden, Haggstrom-Nordin, Nilsson, & Larsson, ). Pornography may contain violent or degrading scenes (Romito & Beltramini, ) or stereotyped depictions of females (Zhou & Bryant, ), which women are reportedly less aroused by than are men (Glascock, ). Important differences may also occur between amateur and professional pornography, with regard to the level of gender inequality featured (Klaassen & Peter, ). As important sex differences may occur with regard to the frequency and form of pornography use (Bohm, Franz, Dekker, & Matthiesen, ; Hald & Stulhofer, ), it would be useful for future studies to directly examine the influence of different types of pornography used by women on their sexually coercive behavior, rather than extrapolate from existing male-oriented research.

Despite efforts to recruit a diverse range of participants, the number of demographic items presented in the questionnaire was restricted, partly due to stringent ethical guidelines; thus, we were unable to examine racial differences in relation to sexual coercion. This may have been interesting to explore as a previous study has found that Asian males report significantly lower rates of sexual coercion victimization in comparison with their Black, White, and Latino counterparts (see French, Tilghman, & Malebranche, ). Other factors that previous studies report as significant mediating factors for sexual coercion in women, and thus are likely to yield valuable results in future research, include the influence of alcohol (Ménard et al., ) and sexual abuse history (Anderson, ; Russell & Oswald, ; ). Alcohol use may be of particular importance given that this study found HPD traits to be significantly associated with sexual exploitation of the intoxicated. To align with other general population research, this study aimed to examine sexually coercive behavior in women with no sex offense charges; despite recruiting participants from community and student populations, this caveat could only be inferred as questions to explicitly measure sex offending history were not included. Thus, future studies with females could directly measure participants’ engagement in criminality or could recruit participants with known sexual offending histories from clinical or forensic populations.

Females’ sexual coercion of males is often considered as less harmful by the general population than the same victimization of women by men (French et al., ; Huitema & Vanwesenbeeck, ; Struckman-Johnson et al., ; Studzinska & Hilton, ). Although male victims of female sexual coercion may also report positive responses to sexual coercion, some studies have reported that 90% of men also report at least one negative response to coercion (Kernsmith & Kernsmith, ) and display significant psychological distress and risk behaviors (French et al., ; Turchik, ; Walker, Archer, & Davis, ). Relatively little research is available, however, to identify factors that influence attribution of blame to female perpetrators. Initial findings suggest that while male perpetrators are perceived to be aggressive, female perpetrators are regarded as promiscuous (Oswald & Russell, ). Additional research would be useful to determine factors that influence perceptions of victimization, victim-reporting or self-identification as a perpetrator or victim. An exploration of sexual coercion experienced by females who identify as LGBTQ is also a worthy avenue of further investigation, as previous studies note this may be prevalent but underreported (e.g., Turell, ; Waterman, Dawson, & Bologna, ). Finally, it is important to emphasize that the current study investigated women’s perpetration of sexually coercive behavior rather than men’s behavior after initial refusal. A range of individual and situational factors may predict responses to sexually coercive behavior such as persuasion that sexual activity is desirable, compliance with unwanted sex, or termination of a relationship (e.g., Nurius & Norris, ). The extent to which women’s sexually coercive behavior results in intercourse remains unclear, however, and future research could consider, for example, whether men experiencing sexual coercion subsequently engage in sex and the extent to which this is unwanted. Similarly, the present study did not assess women’s responses to their partner’s refusal. While it has been reported that women experience more negative reactions to sexual rejection than men (de Graaf & Sandfort, ), those factors impacting on responses to rejection remain unclear.

To conclude, we investigated factors associated with women’s use of sexual coercion. Findings indicate that women’s effort to use pornography was significantly associated with two subtypes of sexual coercion: nonverbal sexual arousal and emotional manipulative and deception to sexually coerce, while HPD traits were associated with exploitation of the intoxicated. Future research should further investigate the influence of pornography effort and HPD traits on aversive sexual behavior and the extent to which these may inform future intervention.

Footnotes

  1. 1.

    “Trending” refers to a topic that experiences a surge in popularity for a limited duration of time, from which e-commerce businesses can extrapolate what is holding consumer interest.

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Statement

This study was approved by the University Ethics Committee in line with British Psychological Society guidelines.

Informed Consent

Participants were able to give informed consent to take part in this study.

References

  1. AlaviHejazi, M., Fatehizade, M., Bahrami, F., & Etemadi, O. (2016). Histrionic women in Iran: A qualitative study of the couple interactive pathology of the women with symptoms of histrionic personality disorder (HPD). Review of European Studies, 9(1), 18–30.  https://doi.org/10.5539/res.v9n1p18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anderson, P. B. (1996). Correlates of college women’s self-reports of heterosexual aggression. Sexual Abuse, 8(2), 121–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Apt, C., & Hurlbert, D. F. (1994). The sexual attitudes, behavior, and relationships of women with histrionic personality disorder. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 20(2), 125–134.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00926239408403423.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Ashton, S., McDonald, K., & Kirkman, M. (2018). Women’s experiences of pornography: A systematic review of research using qualitative methods. Journal of Sex Research, 55(3), 334–347.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2017.1364337.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Blinkhorn, V., Lyons, M., & Almond, L. (2015). The ultimate femme fatale? Narcissism predicts serious and aggressive sexually coercive behavior in females. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 219–223.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bohm, M., Franz, P., Dekker, A., & Matthiesen, S. (2015). Desire and dilemma: Gender differences in German students’ consumption of pornography. Porn Studies, 2(1), 76–92.  https://doi.org/10.1080/23268743.2014.984923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bornstein, R. F., & Malka, I. L. (2009). Dependent and histrionic personality disorders. In P. H. Blaney & T. Millon (Eds.), Oxford textbook of psychopathology (pp. 602–621). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., van Dijk, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Narcissism, sexual refusal, and aggression: Testing a narcissistic reactance model of sexual coercion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(5), 1027–1040.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1027.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Campbell, L., & Kohut, T. (2017). The use and effects of pornography in romantic relationships. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 6–10.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.03.004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Christopher, F. S., Madura, M., & Weaver, L. (1998). Premarital sexual aggressors: A multivariate analysis of social, relational and individual variables. Journal of Marriage and Family, 60(1), 56–69.  https://doi.org/10.2307/353441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. de Graaf, H., & Sandfort, T. G. M. (2004). Gender differences in affective responses to sexual rejection. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33(4), 395–403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Denov, M. S. (2017). Perspectives on female sex offending: A culture of denial. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dorfman, W. I. (2010). Histrionic personality disorder. Corsini encyclopedia of psychology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  15. Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(3), 291–300.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_11.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Erulkar, A. S. (2004). The experience of sexual coercion among young people in Kenya. International Family Planning Perspectives, 30(4), 182–189.  https://doi.org/10.1363/ifpp.30.182.04.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Fisher, W. A., Kohut, T., & Campbell, L. (2017). Patterns of pornography use of men and women in couple relationships, Manuscript in preparation. Department of Psychology, Western University, London, ON, Canada.Google Scholar
  18. French, B. H., Tilghman, J. D., & Malebranche, D. A. (2015). Sexual coercion context and psychosocial correlates among diverse males. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 16(1), 42–53.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gewirtz-Maydan, A. (2017). Why do narcissistic individuals engage in sex? Exploring sexual motives as a mediator for sexual satisfaction and function. Personality and Individual Differences, 105, 7–13.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.09.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Glascock, J. (2005). Degrading content and character sex: Accounting for men and women’s differential reactions to pornography. Communication Reports, 18(1–2), 43–53.  https://doi.org/10.1080/08934210500084230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gonsalves, V. M., Hodges, H., & Scalora, M. J. (2015). Exploring the use of online sexually explicit material: What is the relationship to sexual coercion? Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 22, 207–221.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10720162.2015.1039150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Grayston, A. D., & De Luca, R. V. (1999). Female perpetrators of child sexual abuse: A review of the clinical and empirical literature. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 4(1), 93–106.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1359-1789(98)00014-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grubbs, J. B., Sessoms, J., Wheeler, D. M., & Volk, F. (2010). The Cyber-Pornography Use Inventory: The development of a new assessment instrument. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 17(2), 106–126.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10720161003776166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hald, G. M., & Stulhofer, A. (2016). What types of pornography do people use and do they cluster? Assessing types and categories of pornography consumption in a large-scale online sample. Journal of Sex Research, 53(7), 849–859.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2015.1065953.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Hines, D. A. (2007). Predictors of sexual coercion against women and men: A multilevel, multinational study of university students. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36(3), 403–422.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-006-9141-4.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hoffmann, A. M., & Verona, E. (2018). Psychopathic traits and sexual coercion against relationship partners in men and women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260518754873.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Huitema, A., & Vanwesenbeeck, I. (2016). Attitudes of Dutch citizens towards male victims of sexual coercion by a female perpetrator. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 22(3), 308–322.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13552600.2016.1159343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hyler, S. E. (1994). Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire-4 (PDQ-4). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
  29. Kernsmith, P. D., & Kernmith, R. M. (2009). Female pornography use and sexual coercion perpetration. Deviant Behavior, 30(7), 589–610.  https://doi.org/10.1080/01639620802589798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kernsmith, P. D., & Kernsmith, R. M. (2009). Gender differences in response to sexual coercion. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 19(7), 902–914.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10911350903008098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Khan, R., Brewer, G., Kim, S., & Centifanti, L. C. M. (2017). Students, sex, and psychopathy: Borderline and psychopathy personality traits are differently related to women and men’s use of sexual coercion, partner poaching, and promiscuity. Personality and Individual Differences, 107, 72–77.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.11.027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kjellgren, C., Priebe, G., Svedin, C. G., Mossige, S., & Långström, N. (2011). Female youth who sexually coerce: Prevalence, risk, and protective factors in two national high school surveys. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 8(12), 3354–3362.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01495.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Klaassen, M. J. E., & Peter, J. (2015). Gender (in)equality in internet pornography: A content analysis of popular pornographic internet videos. Journal of Sex Research, 52(7), 721–735.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2014.976781.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Krahé, B., & Berger, A. (2013). Men and women as perpetrators and victims of sexual aggression in heterosexual and same-sex encounters: A study of first-year college students in Germany. Aggressive Behavior, 39(5), 391–404.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21482.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Krahé, B., & Berger, A. (2017). Gendered pathways from child sexual abuse to sexual aggression victimization and perpetration in adolescence and young adulthood. Child Abuse and Neglect, 63, 261–272.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.10.004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Krahé, B., Berger, A., Vanwesenbeeck, I., Bianchi, G., Chliaoutakis, J., Fernández-Fuertes, A. A.,… & Hellemans, S. (2015). Prevalence and correlates of young people’s sexual aggression perpetration and victimisation in 10 European countries: A multi-level analysis. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 17(6), 682–699.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13691058.2014.989265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Krahé, B., Waizenhöfer, E., & Möller, I. (2003). Women’s sexual aggression against men: Prevalence and predictors. Sex Roles, 49(5–6), 219–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Lamkin, J., Lavner, J. A., & Shaffer, A. (2017). Narcissism and observed communication in couples. Personality and Individual Differences, 105, 224–228.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.09.046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Logan, C. (2008). Sexual deviance in females: Psychopathology and theory. In D. R. Laws & W. T. O’Donohue (Eds.), Sexual deviance: Theory, assessment, and treatment (pp. 486–507). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  40. Mattebo, M., Tyden, T., Haggstrom-Nordin, E., Nilsson, K. W., & Larsson, M. (2016). Pornography consumption among adolescent girls in Sweden. European Journal of Contraception & Reproductive Health Care, 21(4), 295–302.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13625187.2016.1186268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Ménard, K. S., Hall, G. C. N., Phung, A. H., Ghebrial, M. F. E., & Martin, L. (2003). Gender differences in sexual harassment and coercion in college students: Developmental, individual, and situational determinants. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18(10), 1222–1239.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260503256654.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Mouilso, E. R., & Calhoun, K. S. (2016). Personality and perpetration: Narcissism among college sexual assault perpetrators. Violence Against Women, 22(10), 1228–1242.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801215622575.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Muñoz, L. C., Khan, R., & Cordwell, L. (2011). Sexually coercive tactics used by university students: A clear role for primary psychopathy. Journal of Personality Disorders, 25(1), 28–40.  https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2011.25.1.28.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Nurius, P. S., & Norris, J. (1996). A cognitive ecological model of women’s response to male sexual coercion in dating. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 8(1–2), 117–139.  https://doi.org/10.1300/J056v08n0109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. O’Sullivan, L. F., Byers, E. S., & Finkelman, L. (1998). A comparison of male and female college students’ experiences of sexual coercion. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22(2), 177–195.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1998.tb00149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Oswald, D. L., & Russell, B. L. (2006). Perceptions of sexual coercion in heterosexual dating relationships: The role of aggressor gender and tactics. Journal of Sex Research, 43(1), 87–95.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490609552302.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Pornhub Insights. (2018). 2017 in review. Retrieved January 22 2018, from https://www.pornhub.com/insights/2017-year-in-review.
  48. Rissel, C., Richters, J., de Visser, R. O., McKee, A., Yeung, A., & Caruana, T. (2017). A profile of pornography users in Australia: Findings from the Second Australian Study of Health and Relationships. Journal of Sex Research, 54(2), 227–240.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2016.1191597.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Romito, P., & Beltramini, L. (2015). Factors associated with exposure to violent or degrading pornography among high school students. Journal of School Nursing, 31(4), 280–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Russell, T. D., Doan, C. M., & King, A. R. (2017). Sexually violent women: The PID-5, everyday sadism, and adversarial sexual attitudes predict female sexual aggression and coercion against male victims. Personality and Individual Differences, 111, 242–249.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Russell, B. L., & Oswald, D. L. (2001). Strategies and dispositional correlates of sexual coercion perpetuated by women: An exploratory investigation. Sex Roles, 45(1–2), 103–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Russell, B. L., & Oswald, D. L. (2002). Sexual coercion and victimization of college men: The role of love styles. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 17(3), 273–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Ryan, K. M., Weikel, K., & Sprechini, G. (2008). Gender differences in narcissism and courtship violence in dating couples. Sex Roles, 58(11–12), 802–813.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9403-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Schatzel-Murphy, E. A., Harris, D. A., Knight, R. A., & Milburn, M. A. (2009). Sexual coercion in men and women: Similar behaviors, different predictors. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38(6), 974–986.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-009-9481-y.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Schneider, J. P. (1994). Sex addiction: Controversy within mainstream addiction medicine, diagnosis based on the DSM-III-R, and physician case histories. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 1(1), 19–44.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10720169408400025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Ševčíková, A., & Daneback, K. (2014). Online pornography use in adolescence: Age and gender differences. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(6), 674–686.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2014.926808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Stanley, N., Barter, C., Wood, M., Aghtaie, N., Larkins, C., Lanau, A., & Overlien, C. (2018). Pornography, sexual coercion and abuse and sexting in young people’s intimate relationships: A European study. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 33(19), 2919–2944.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260516633204.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Stinson, F. S., Dawson, D. A., Goldstein, R. B., Chou, S. P., Huang, B., Smith, S. M., … Grant, B. F. (2008). Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV narcissistic personality disorder: Results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(7), 1033–1045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Stone, M. H. (2005). Borderline and histrionic personality disorders: A review. In M. Maj, H. S. Akiskal, J. E. Mezzich, & A. Okasha (Eds.), Personality disorders (pp. 201–231). Chichester, England: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Struckman-Johnson, C., Struckman-Johnson, D., & Anderson, P. B. (2003). Tactics of sexual coercion: When men and women won’t take no for an answer. Journal of Sex Research, 40(1), 76–86.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490309552168.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Studzinska, A. M., & Hilton, D. (2017). Minimization of male suffering: Social perception of victims and perpetrators of opposite-sex sexual coercion. Sexuality Research and Social Policy, 14(1), 87–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Torgersen, S., Kringlen, E., & Cramer, V. (2001). The prevalence of personality disorders in a community sample. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(6), 590–596.  https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.58.6.590.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Torgersen, S., Lygren, S., Øien, P. A., Skre, I., Onstad, S., Edvardsen, J., … Kringlen, E. (2000). A twin study of personality disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 41(6), 416–425.  https://doi.org/10.1053/comp.2000.16560.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Turchik, J. A. (2012). Sexual victimization among male college students: Assault severity, sexual functioning, and health risk behaviors. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 13(3), 243–255.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Turell, S. C. (2000). A descriptive analysis of same-sex relationship violence for a diverse sample. Journal of Family Violence, 15(3), 281–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Tylka, T. L., & Kroon Van Diest, A. M. (2015). You looking at her “hot” body may not be “cool” for me: Integrating male partners’ pornography use into objectification theory for women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 39(1), 67–84.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684314521784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Visser, R. O., Smith, A., Rissel, C. E., Richters, J., & Grulich, A. E. (2003). Sex in Australia: Experiences of sexual coercion among a representative sample of Adults. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health, 27(2), 198–203.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-842X.2003.tb00808.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Walker, J., Archer, J., & Davies, M. (2005). Effects of rape on men: A descriptive analysis. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34(1), 69–80.  https://doi.org/10.1007/a10508-005-1001-0.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Waterman, C. K., Dawson, L. J., & Bologna, M. J. (1989). Sexual coercion in gay male and lesbian relationships: Predictors and implications for support services. Journal of Sex Research, 26(1), 118–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (2007). Plate tectonics in the classification of personality disorder: Shifting to a dimensional model. American Psychologist, 62(2), 71–83.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.2.71.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Wright, P. J., Bae, S., & Funk, M. (2013). United States women and pornography through four decades: Exposure, attitudes, behaviors, individual differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42(7), 1131–1144.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0116-y.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Wright, M. O. D., Norton, D. L., & Matusek, J. A. (2010). Predicting verbal coercion following sexual refusal during a hookup: Diverging gender patterns. Sex Roles, 62(9–10), 647–660.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9763-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Wright, P. J., Tokunaga, R. S., & Kraus, A. (2016). A meta-analysis of pornography consumption and actual acts of sexual aggression in general population studies. Journal of Communication, 66(1), 183–205.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.com.12201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Yost, M. R., & Zurbriggen, E. L. (2006). Gender differences in the enactment of sociosexuality: An examination of implicit social motives, sexual fantasies, coercive sexual attitudes, and aggressive sexual behavior. Journal of Sex Research, 43(2), 163–173.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490609552311.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Zeigler-Hill, V., Besser, A., Morag, J., & Campbell, W. K. (2016). The Dark Triad and sexual harassment proclivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 89, 47–54.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Zhou, Y., & Bryant, P. (2016). Lotus Blossom or Dragon Lady: A content analysis of “Asian Women” online pornography. Sexuality and Culture, 20, 1083–1100.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12119-016-9375-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Zurbriggen, E. L. (2000). Social motives and cognitive power-sex associations: Predictors of aggressive sexual behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(3), 559–581.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.78.3.559.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar