Low 2D:4D values are associated with video game addiction (2013)

PLoS One. 2013 Nov 13;8(11):e79539.

doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079539. eCollection 2013.

Kornhuber J1, Zenses EM, Lenz B, Stoessel C, Bouna-Pyrrou P, Rehbein F, Kliem S, Mößle T.

Abstract

Androgen-dependent signaling regulates the growth of the fingers on the human hand during embryogenesis. A higher androgen load results in lower 2D:4D (second digit to fourth digit) ratio values. Prenatal androgen exposure also impacts brain development. 2D:4D values are usually lower in males and are viewed as a proxy of male brain organization. Here, we quantified video gaming behavior in young males. We found lower mean 2D:4D values in subjects who were classified according to the CSAS-II as having at-risk/addicted behavior (n = 27) compared with individuals with unproblematic video gaming behavior (n = 27). Thus, prenatal androgen exposure and a hyper-male brain organization, as represented by low 2D:4D values, are associated with problematic video gaming behavior. These results may be used to improve the diagnosis, prediction, and prevention of video game addiction.

Introduction

A high prenatal androgen load, induced either by enhanced hormone levels or more sensitive androgen signal transduction pathways, results in a longer fourth digit (4D) relative to the second digit (2D) in the adult human hand [1]. Therefore, 2D:4D values are considered to be sexually dimorphic, with values usually lower in males compared with females [2][4]. Additionally, the prenatal androgen load has an organizing effect on brain structure and function [5]. As a result, 2D:4D values are associated with a wide range of male/female behavioral phenotypes. Low 2D:4D values are associated, for example, with autistic features [6], [7]; attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [8], [9]; athletic performance [10], [11]; spatial abilities [12][15]; abstract reasoning [16]; numeric abilities [17][19]; cooperativeness, pro-social behavior, and fairness [20], [21]; number of life-time sexual partners [22]; and reproductive success [23]. The evidence linking the prenatal androgen load with low 2D:4D values and behavioral traits has recently been reviewed [24], [25].

We have previously shown lower mean 2D:4D values in patients with alcohol dependence [26], a substance-related addictive disorder with a higher prevalence in males than females [27], [28]. In this study, we aimed to analyze whether low 2D:4D values are also associated with addictive video gaming behavior, which is a non-substance-related addiction behavior. Severe gaming behavior occurs much more frequently in males compared with females [29][32] and is associated with sensation seeking [33] and ADHD [34]. Pathological video gaming may be viewed as a hyper-male behavior. Therefore, we hypothesized that males with pathological video gaming behavior may have been prenatally exposed to a higher androgen load, as indicated by their lower 2D:4D values.

Methods

This study is part of the Finger-Length in Psychiatry (FLIP) project of the Erlangen Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy as well as the longitudinal interview study module of the project entitled “Internet and Video Game Addiction – diagnostics, epidemiology, etiopathogenesis, treatment and prevention” of the Criminology Research Institute of Lower Saxony. The FLIP-Project was realized as an add-on at the second measurement occasion (t2) of the longitudinal interview study. This investigation has been conducted according to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (Ethics Committee of the German Psychological Society [Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychologie]). Written informed consent was obtained after providing a complete description of the study to all of the subjects.

Between February and December 2011, 70 subjects participated at the first measurement occasion (t1) of the longitudinal interview study (they were originally chosen from an overall 1,092 prospective participants which were recruited via schools, universities, internet forums, newspapers, and counseling centers). Prerequisites for study participation at t1: male, 18-21 years old, habitual video gamers with either more than 2.5 hours of gaming a day or a Video Game Addition Scale (CSAS-II) score > 41 [29], see below). From March 2012 to January 2013, 64 participants could be interviewed again at the t2 follow-up of the longitudinal interview study. At this measurement occasion a total of 54 subjects agreed to additionally participate in the FLIP project. These 54 subjects can be characterized as follows: 53 Caucasian, 1 Asian. Mean age at t1 was 18.9 years (SD = 1.1). 24 of the participants had a higher educational level (Abitur or higher), another 24 had secondary schooling (Realschule), 5 reported lower secondary schooling (Hauptschule) and one no graduation.

Video game addiction was assessed using the CSAS II [29] at t1. The CSAS II is based on the Internet Addiction Scale ISS-20 [35], [36], which has been extended and adapted to assess video game addiction. The CSAS-II consists of 14 items (4-point scale: 1  =  incorrect to 4  =  absolutely true) and covers the dimensions preoccupation/salience (4 items), conflict (4 items), loss of control (2 items), withdrawal symptoms (2 items), and tolerance (2 items). The items of the CSAS-II show high face validity, and the instrument demonstrates good convergent validity for subjective self-evaluation measures of video game addiction [29], [30]. Additionally, the CSAS-II-classification of video game addiction is not only associated with excessive gaming behavior but also identifies different measures of functional level and well-being [29], [30], [37]. The following diagnostic cut-offs are used: 14–34 =  unproblematic, 35–41 = at risk of becoming addicted, and 42–56 =  addicted.

According to CSAS-II classification, which is going beyond mere gaming times, 27 participants were classified as unproblematic video gamers, 17 as at risk of becoming addicted and 10 as addicted. Because of the small number of subjects investigated, the two groups “at risk of becoming addicted” and “addicted” were joined for analyses. Thus, two CSAS-II categories (unproblematic vs. at risk/addicted) with each 27 subjects were investigated in this study.

Psychological problems and symptoms of psychopathology were assessed at t1 using the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) [38]. The subscales interpersonal sensitivity (T = 52.26, SD = 11.81), depression (T = 53.98, SD = 11.64), anxiety (T = 54.30, SD = 10.23), and hostility (T = 52.20, SD  = 11.56) were used as control variables in the multivariate analyses. In addition, ADHD symptomatology, which was also used as control variable, was assessed using the ADHD-Screening for adults (ADHS-E; T = 54.02, SD = 8.79) [39].

An Avision IS1000 flatbed scanner (Hsinchu, Taiwan) was used to scan the participants’ hands at t2. To increase accuracy, small marks were drawn on the basal creases of each of the participants’ index and ring fingers before scanning. Both hands were scanned at the same time, with palms down, in black-white mode. We used the GNU Image Manipulation Program (GIMP, version 2.8.4; www.gimp.org) to measure the lengths of the index (2D) and ring (4D) fingers from the hand scans. This technique provides good reliability [40]. The total length of the second and fourth digit of the left and right hands was quantified from the middle of the basal crease to the tip of the finger and was determined in units of pixels using the GIMP “measure” tool. The measurements were performed by three independent individuals who were blind to the hypothesis and blind to the diagnostic category. Mean values of the three measurements were calculated for the second and fourth digit.

Statistical analyses were computed using IBM SPSS 19 (Armonk, New York, USA) and the R software.

Results

Differences in age between the unproblematic and at risk/addicted groups were analyzed by the Student’s t-test; differences in educational level by the Fisheŕs exact test for contingency tables larger than 2×2 [41], [42]. Both of the CSAS II groups (unproblematic vs. at risk/addicted) were well matched with respect to age (t = 1.544, p = 0.129) and educational level (p = 0.381; see Table 1).

Table 1 

Mean 2D:4D and Dr–l values in individuals with unproblematic vs. at-risk/addicted video gaming behavior.

The reliability of the three measurements of the fingers was calculated for each finger separately for the right and left hand using the two-way random intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) [43]. ICCs were also calculated for 2D:4D ratios and right 2D:4D–left 2D:4D (Dr–l) values. The reliability of the three raters was high for both the right hand (2D: ICC  = 0.995; 4D: ICC  = 0.995; 2D:4D: ICC  = 0.944), the left hand (2D: ICC  = 0.996; 4D: ICC  = 0.994; 2D:4D: ICC  = 0.937), and the arithmetic mean (2D:4D: ICC  = 0.961). The reliability of the Dr–l values was also high (ICC  = 0.764).

Deviation from normal distribution was tested by the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. The 2D:4D (arithmetic mean: Z = 0.931, p = 0.351, left hand: Z = 0.550, p = 0.923, right hand: Z = 0.913, p = 0.375) and Dr–l (Z = 1.082, p = 0.193) values did not deviate from a normal distribution. The mean 2D:4D and Dr–l values are presented in Table 1.

Differences in 2D:4D and Dr–1 values depending on educational level were tested for the unproblematic and at risk/addicted group by the Kruskal Wallis test. Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated. The correlation between 2D:4D values for the right vs. left hand was 0.788 (p < 0.01). 2D:4D and Dr–l values did not differ significantly depending on educational level within the unproblematic (arithmetic mean: χ2(2, N = 54)  = 1.831, p = 0.400, left hand: χ2(2, N = 54)  = 2.247, p = 0.325, right hand: χ 2(2, N = 54)  = 2.005, p = 0.367, Dr–1: χ2(2, N = 54)  = 0.637, p = 0.747) and at risk/addicted group (arithmetic mean: χ2(3, N = 54)  = 3.363, p = 0.339, left hand: χ2(3, N = 54)  = 2.139, p = 0.544, right hand: χ2(3, N = 54)  = 3.348, p = 0.341, Dr–1: χ2(3, N = 54) = 0.460, p = 0.928).

Associations between measures of 2D:4D (left hand, right hand, arithmetic mean, Dr–1) and video game addiction (unproblematic vs. at risk/addicted group) were tested by a non-parametric multivariate approach based on the principle of recursive partitioning, i.e. conditional inference trees (C-Tree; [44], [45]). Controlling for interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility and ADHD, comparable to a stepwise regression non-significant predictors are excluded. Using the C-Tree algorithm the global hypothesis of independence between any of the input variables and the response variable is tested using a permutation test framework [46]. For metric variables the C-Tree algorithm implements a binary split in the selected input variable. To determine the “best” binary split, several split criteria are provided (e.g., “Gini importance”, “impurity of node” or “entropy”). However, most splitting criteria are not applicable to correlated response variables or response variables measured with different scale formats (e.g., metric and nominal). We therefore utilized the permutation test framework described by Hothorn et al. [47] (p. 6, equation 3). Since permutation tests derive the p-values from sample-specific permutation distributions of the test statistics, only p-values are reported. The R package “party” (a laboratory for recursive partitioning; [47], [48]) was used for this analysis.

In the multivariate non-parametric analyses, measures of 2D:4D (arithmetic mean, left hand, right hand) were associated with video game addiction (unproblematic vs. at risk/addicted group) when controlling for interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility and ADHD: 1. Study participants with a mean 2D:4D ratio lower than 0.966 showed a significantly higher risk of being video game addicted (p = 0.027, d  = 0.71). 2. For the left hand, study participants with a 2D:4D ratio lower than 0.982 showed a significantly higher risk of being video game addicted (p = 0.013, d = 0.93). 3. For the right hand study participants with a 2D:4D ratio lower than 0.979 showed a significantly higher risk of being video game addicted on the level of p < 0.10 (p = 0.095, d  = 0.66). Moreover, study participants who additionally scored higher than 60 (T-score) on the ADHS-E were particularly at risk (p = 0.078, d = 0.69). No significant association was found for Dr–1 (p = 0.127). Figures 1a to 1c illustrate the risk of video game addiction for the mean 2D:4D, as well as the left and right 2D:4D values in C-Tree. Independent of the reported 2D:4D cut off values mean group differences in measures of 2D:4D between unproblematic and at risk/addicted can be observed, which is exemplified for mean 2D:4D in figure 2 using the same analysis with reversed dependent and independent variables. Together, these results indicate that at-risk/addicted video gamers have smaller 2D:4D ratios.

Figure 1 

Conditional Inference tree plots.
Figure 2 

Conditional Inference tree plot.

To estimate the value of the 2D:4D ratio as a diagnostic test for the discrimination of video game-addicted/at risk individuals vs. controls with unproblematic gaming behavior, we used a ROC analysis to calculate AUC values, as well as sensitivity and specificity at the Youden point [49] (the point on the ROC curve where the sum of sensitivity and specificity is maximized). The ROC analysis shows that the diagnostic accuracy of the 2D:4D ratio of the left hand is highest (AUC 0.704, sensitivity 0.852, specificity 0.556), followed by that of the right hand (AUC 0.639, sensitivity 0.815, specificity 0.481). According to Hanley and McNeil [50] we checked for differences in paired AUCs with no significant result (Z = 1.147, p = 0.25).

Discussion

This is the first investigation linking prenatal androgen exposure with addictive video gaming behavior. In this study, we found low mean 2D:4D values in subjects with at risk and addicted video gaming behavior. Effect sizes larger than d = 0.66 point to a moderate to strong effect [51]. No other considered predictors, except symptoms of ADHD for the right 2D:4D calculations were statistically significant in the multivariate nonparametric analyses. The observed association between at-risk/addicted video gaming and low 2D:4D values can be interpreted in several ways. (1) A small 2D:4D value directly induces addictive gaming behavior; however, there is no evidence in the literature to support this possibility. (2) Addictive gaming behavior directly induces low 2D:4D values. However, this possibility is unlikely because previous studies have proven that 2D:4D values remain constant throughout life after birth [52]. (3) A common mechanism is responsible for both low 2D:4D values and addictive gaming behavior. Based on the existing data, such a factor provides the most likely explanation. The results of the 2D:4D C-tree calculations with an additional explanatory power of symptoms of ADHD also support this explanation. Addictive gaming is more frequent in males [29][32] and is associated with ADHD [34] and sensation seeking [33]. All of these features have previously been linked to low 2D:4D values. One common reason for these associations appears to be a high androgen load during pregnancy.

Understanding the pathways leading from enhanced prenatal testosterone to game addiction will be crucial for defining potential policies targeting video game addiction. Prenatal testosterone may induce addictive behavior through several channels including the following: (1) Prenatal testosterone abundance modulates the mesolimbic reward system [53] thereby potentially affecting addictive gaming behavior in adults. (2) The specific rules of the cyber world as compared to the real world might compensate limitations in social interaction abilities caused by high prenatal testosterone load. Higher fetal testosterone levels have been shown to reduce empathy and the capacity to decode emotional facial expression, i.e. to understand what other people think and feel [54]. In line with that, lower 2D:4D values were related to reduced empathy in males [55]. Moreover, a smaller 2D:4D is linked to more indiscriminate social suspicion [56]. Thus, high prenatal testosterone might cause interpersonal problems and social isolation and, thereby, entail pathological video gaming behavior as a coping strategy. (3) It is likely that the abilities that facilitate or impede computer use modulate a person’s risk of developing video game addiction. Thus, our results concur with previous findings linking low 2D:4D with Java-related programming skills and high 2D:4D values with computer-related anxiety [57].

Previously, we found low mean 2D:4D values in individuals with alcohol addiction [26], a substance-related addiction disorder. It is noteworthy that low 2D:4D values also occur in individuals with a video gaming addiction, which is a non-substance related addictive disorder that is more prevalent in males than females. This result underscores the similarity between substance-related addiction and internet gaming addiction [58]. According to the DSM-5, internet gaming disorder is included in the appendix as a subject for further research. The literature suggests a biological basis of computer and internet gaming addiction [59][61]. The results presented here provide further evidence for a biological basis of internet gaming addiction and, thus, offer an argument for its classification as an addiction disorder.

Many phenomena have been linked to low 2D:4D values, most of which are compatible with the hyper-male brain hypothesis. Thus, low 2D:4D values may be regarded as a proxy of the endophenotype “hyper-male brain organization”. However, the precise effect of a high prenatal androgen load on the life of an individual and on that individual’s future adult behavior must also depend on additional variables and influences. The specific behavioral phenotype evolving as a result of the hyper-male brain organization most likely depends on a myriad of genetic and environmental factors that are experienced over an individual’s lifetime. Therefore, the presence of low 2D:4D values does not suggest a specific diagnosis or prognosis for any single individual. However, knowledge of 2D:4D values may aid in improving an individual’s diagnosis and prognosis associated with different problematic behaviors and disorders when used in combination with other markers.

These results may have important implications for the diagnosis, prevention, and consequences of addictive gaming. A low 2D:4D value alone is not diagnostic of addictive gaming, but this factor may facilitate the diagnosis when used in conjunction with other markers. A low 2D:4D value may help to identify individuals who are at risk for future development of addictive gaming and, thus, may facilitate prevention. Several attempts have been made to predict the development of internet gaming addiction in individuals [62][67]. A low 2D:4D value is a novel trait marker; combined with other markers, its use may improve the prediction of the future development or the current diagnosis of internet gaming addiction. Such improved prediction models may enable the development of effective preventive strategies.

We investigated individuals in a narrow age range; furthermore, the mean age did not differ between the two groups. In previous studies, age was, if at all, only marginally associated with 2D:4D values [68]. Therefore, age was not considered in the non-parametric analyses. Notably, education level did not differ between the two groups investigated in this study.

In additional analyses we also checked for a possible non-monotonic relationship between measures of 2D:4D and video game addiction using the CSAS-II sum score, as this has been reported for example for measures of 2D:4D and altruism [69]. The linear regression analyses revealed no significant linear, quadratic or combined trend – also with logarithmic transformation of the arithmetic mean (see [69]). Furthermore, these results were confirmed by non-parametric regression analyses [70], [71]. Together these analyses support the assumption to regard video game addiction as a categorical construct with qualitative distinct categories (unproblematic vs. problematic, i.e. at risk/addicted), such as previously reported for alcohol addiction [72].

The time spent with video gaming alone does not define addiction. For the diagnosis “video game addiction” further criteria have to be met: preoccupation, withdrawal, tolerance, loss of control, and continued use despite negative consequences. A strength of this study is the composition of the participants. All of the participants spent some time each day with video gaming, but only half of the participants had additional criteria defining them being at risk/addicted (as assessed by CSAS-II). Our results thus define 2D:4D as a risk factor specifically related to video game addiction, not just to video game playing per se.

Several study limitations should be noted. We used a mono-centric, cross-sectional, case-control design, which allows the detection of associations only, without causal relationships. Additionally, we investigated only males, and the sample group was relatively small. The strong effect size of 2D:4D on video gaming addiction probably enabled the detection of group differences despite the relatively low number of subjects. In our previous study, we also found a strong effect size relating 2D:4D to alcohol addiction [26]. Because of the well-known sex differences in addictive behavior [5], future studies should include females, should include other ethnicities and should also include a larger sample size.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all our participants, our student assistant Julia Weberling, and our IT-system administrator André Liedtke.

Funding Statement

Funding for this study was provided by intramural grants from the University Hospital of the Friedrich-Alexander-University of Erlangen-Nuremberg and by the Ministry for Science and Culture of Lower Saxony. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

1. Zheng Z, Cohn MJ (2011) Developmental basis of sexually dimorphic digit ratios. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108: 16289–16294 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
2. Manning JT, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones DI (1998) The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Hum Reprod 13: 3000–3004 [PubMed]
3. Manning JT, Bundred PE, Flanagan BF (2002) The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a proxy for transactivation activity of the androgen receptor gene? Med Hypotheses 59: : 334–336. S0306987702001810 [pii]. [PubMed]
4. Hönekopp J, Watson S (2010) Meta-analysis of digit ratio 2D:4D shows greater sex difference in the right hand. Am J Hum Biol 22: 619–63010.1002/ajhb.21054 []. [PubMed]
5. Lenz B, Müller CP, Stoessel C, Sperling W, Biermann T, et al. (2012) Sex hormone activity in alcohol addiction: Integrating organizational and activational effects. Prog Neurobiol 96: 136–163 [PubMed]
6. Hönekopp J (2012) Digit ratio 2D:4D in relation to autism spectrum disorders, empathizing, and systemizing: a quantitative review. Autism Res 5: 221–23010.1002/aur.1230 []. [PubMed]
7. Teatero ML, Netley C (2013) A critical review of the research on the extreme male brain theory and digit ratio (2D4D). J Autism Dev Disord. 10.1007/s10803-013-1819-6 []. [PubMed]
8. Stevenson JC, Everson PM, Williams DC, Hipskind G, Grimes M, et al. (2007) Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and digit ratios in a college sample. Am J Hum Biol 19: 41–5010.1002/ajhb.20571 []. [PubMed]
9. Martel MM, Gobrogge KL, Breedlove SM, Nigg JT (2008) Masculinized finger-length ratios of boys, but not girls, are associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav Neurosci 122: 273–2812008-03769-003 [pii];10.1037/0735-7044.122.2.273 []. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
10. Hönekopp J, Schuster M (2010) A meta-analysis on 2D:4D and athletic prowess: substantial relationships but neither hand out-predics the other. Pers Individ Dif 48: 4–10
11. Hönekopp J, Manning T, Müller C (2006) Digit ratio (2D:4D) and physical fitness in males and females: Evidence for effects of prenatal androgens on sexually selected traits. Horm Behav 49: 545–549 [PubMed]
12. Chai XJ, Jacobs LF (2012) Digit ratio predicts sense of direction in women. PLoS ONE 7: e3281610.1371/journal.pone.0032816 [];PONE-D-11-11328 [pii]. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
13. Puts DA, McDaniel MA, Jordan CL, Breedlove SM (2008) Spatial ability and prenatal androgens: Meta-analyses of congenital adrenal hyperplasia and digit ratio (2D:4D) studies. Arch Sex Behav 37: 100–111 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
14. Peters M, Manning JT, Reimers S (2007) The effects of sex, sexual orientation, and digit ratio (2D:4D) on mental rotation performance. Arch Sex Behav 36: 251–260 [PubMed]
15. Sanders G, Bereczkei T, Csatho A, Manning J (2005) The ratio of the 2nd to 4th finger length predicts spatial ability in men but not women. Cortex 41: 789–795 [PubMed]
16. Brañas-Garza P, Rustichini A (2011) Organizing effects of testosterone and economic behavior: not just risk taking. PLoS ONE 6: e2984210.1371/journal.pone.0029842 [];PONE-D-11-09556 [pii]. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
17. Brookes H, Neave N, Hamilton C, Fink B (2007) Digit ratio (2D:4D) and lateralization for numerical quantification. J Individual Differences 28: 55–63
18. Kempel P, Gohlke B, Klempau J, Zinsberger P, Reuter M, et al. (2005) Second to fourth digit length, testosterone and spatial ability. Intelligence 33: 215–230
19. Luxen MF, Buunk BP (2005) Second-to-fourth digit ratio related to verbal and numerical intelligence and the Big Five. Pers Individ Dif 39: 959–966
20. Millet K, Dewitte S (2006) Second to fourth digit ratio and cooperative behavior. Biol Psychol 71: 111–115 [PubMed]
21. Millet K, Dewitte S (2009) The presence of aggression cues inverts the relation between digit ratio (2D:4D) and prosocial behaviour in a dictator game. Br J Psychol 100: 151–162300676 [pii];10.1348/000712608X324359 []. [PubMed]
22. Hönekopp J, Voracek M, Manning JT (2006) 2nd to 4th digit ratio (2D:4D) and number of sex partners: evidence for effects of prenatal testosterone in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31: 30–37 [PubMed]
23. Manning JT, Fink B (2008) Digit ratio (2D:4D), dominance, reproductive success, asymmetry, and sociosexuality in the BBC Internet Study. Am J Hum Biol 20: 451–46110.1002/ajhb.20767 []. [PubMed]
24. Hönekopp J, Bartholdt L, Beier L, Liebert A (2007) Second to fourth digit length ratio (2D:4D) and adult sex hormone levels: New data and a meta-analytic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32: 313–321S0306-4530(07)00035-2 [pii];10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.01.007 []. [PubMed]
25. Breedlove SM (2010) Minireview: Organizational hypothesis: instances of the fingerpost. Endocrinology 151: 4116–4122en.2010-0041 [pii];10.1210/en.2010-0041 []. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
26. Kornhuber J, Erhard G, Lenz B, Kraus T, Sperling W, et al. (2011) Low digit ratio 2D:4D in alcohol dependent patients. PLoS ONE 6: e1933210.1371/journal.pone.0019332 []. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
27. Jackson CP, Matthews G (1988) The prediction of habitual alcohol use from alcohol related expectancies and personality. Alcohol Alcohol 23: 305–314 [PubMed]
28. Lex BW (1991) Some gender differences in alcohol and polysubstance users. Health Psychol 10: 121–132 [PubMed]
29. Rehbein F, Kleimann M, Mößle T (2010) Prevalence and risk factors of video game dependency in adolescence: Results of a german nationwide survey. Cyberpsychol Behav Social Networking 13: 269–277 [PubMed]
30. Rehbein F, Mößle T, Arnaud N, Rumpf HJ (2013) [Video game and internet addiction: The current state of research]. Nervenarzt 84: 569–57510.1007/s00115-012-3721-4 []. [PubMed]
31. Wenzel HG, Bakken IJ, Johansson A, Götestam KG, Oren A (2009) Excessive computer game playing among Norwegian adults: self-reported consequences of playing and association with mental health problems. Psychol Rep 105: 1237–1247 [PubMed]
32. Wölfling K, Thalemann R, Grüsser-Sinopoli SM (2008) Computerspielsucht: Ein psychpathologischer Symptomkomplex im Jugendalter. Psychiatr Prax 35: 226–232 [PubMed]
33. Lin SSJ, Tsai CC (2013) Sensation seeking and internet dependence of Taiwanese high school adolescents. Comput Human Behav 18: 411–426
34. Weinstein A, Weizman A (2012) Emerging association between addictive gaming and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 14: 590–59710.1007/s11920-012-0311-x []. [PubMed]
35. Hahn A, Jerusalem M (2001) Internetsucht: Reliabilität und Validität in der Online-Forschung. In: Theobald A, Dreyer M, Starsetzki T, editors. Handbuch zur Online-Marktforschung. Beiträg aus Wissenschaft und Praxis. Wiesbaden: Babler. pp. 211–234.
36. Hahn A, Jerusalem M (2010) Die Internetsuchtskala (ISS): Psychometrische Eigenschaften und Validität. In: Mücken D, Teske A, Rehbein F, Te Wildt B, editors. Prävention, Diagnostik und Therapie von Computerspielabhängigkeit. Lengerich: Pabst Science Publisher. pp. 185–204.
37. Rehbein F, Mößle T, Jukschat N, Zenses EM (2011) Zur psychosozialen Belastung exzessiver und abhängiger Computerspieler im Jugend- und Erwachsenenalter. Suchttherapie 12: 64–71
38. Franke GH (2000) Brief Symptom Inventory von L.R. Derogatis (Kurzform der SCL-90-R) – Deutsche Version. Göttingen: Beltz Test GmbH.
39. Schmidt A, Petermann F (2010) ADHS-E ADHS Screening für Erwachsene. München: Pearson-Verlag.
40. Bailey AA, Hurd PL (2005) Finger length ratio (2D:4D) correlates with physical aggression in men but not in women. Biol Psychol 68: 215–222 [PubMed]
41. Clarkson DB, Fan Y, Joe H (1993) A remark on algorithm 643: FEXACT: An algorithm for performing Fisher’s Exact Text in r x c contingency tables. ACM Transactions on Mathematical Software 19: 484–488
42. Mehta CR, Patel NR (1986) Algorithms 643. FEXACT: A fortran subroutine for Fisher’s Exact Test on unordered r*c contingency tables. ACM Transactions on Mathematical Software 12: 154–161
43. Müller R, Büttner P (1994) A critical discussion of intraclass correlation coefficients. Stat Med 13: 2465–2476 [PubMed]
44. Strobl C, Malley J, Tutz G (2009) An introduction to recursive partitioning: rationale, application, and characteristics of classification and regression trees, bagging, and random forests. Psychol Methods 14: 323–3482009-22665-002 [pii];10.1037/a0016973 []. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
45. Hothorn T, Hornik K, Zeileis A (2006) Unbiased recursive partitioning: a conditional inference framework. J Comput Graphical Stat 15: 651e674
46. Strasser H, Weber C (1999) On the asymptotic theory of permutation statistics. Mathematical Methods of Statistics 8: 220e250
47. Hothorn T, Hornik K, Zeileis A (2010) party: A laboratory for recursive partytioning. Available: http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.168.2941&rep=rep1&type=pdf Accessed 2013 Oct 5.
48. Hothorn T, Hornik K, Strobl C, Zeileis A (2013) A laboratory for recursive partitioning. Available: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/party/party.pdf Accessed 2013 Oct 5.
49. Youden WJ (1950) Index for rating diagnostic tests. Cancer 3: 32–35 [PubMed]
50. Hanley JA, McNeil BJ (1982) The meaning and use of the area under a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve. Radiology 143: 29–36 [PubMed]
51. Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, New York: Erlbaum.
52. Malas MA, Dogan S, Evcil EH, Desdicioglu K (2006) Fetal development of the hand, digits and digit ratio (2D:4D). Early Hum Dev 82: 469–475 [PubMed]
53. Lombardo MV, Ashwin E, Auyeung B, Chakrabarti B, Lai MC, et al. (2012) Fetal programming effects of testosterone on the reward system and behavioral approach tendencies in humans. Biol Psychiatry 72: 839–847S0006-3223(12)00499-4 [pii];10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.05.027 []. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
54. Chapman E, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, et al. (2006) Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the empathy quotient (EQ) and the “reading the mind in the eyes” test. Soc Neurosci 1: 135–148759346795 [pii];10.1080/17470910600992239 []. [PubMed]
55. Von Horn A, Bäckman L, Davidsson T, Hansen S (2010) Empathizing, systemizing and finger length ratio in a Swedish sample. Scand J Psychol 51: 31–37SJOP725 [pii];10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00725.x []. [PubMed]
56. De Neys W, Hopfensitz A, Bonnefon JF (2013) Low second-to-fourth digit ratio predicts indiscriminate social suspicion, not improved trustworthiness detection. Biol Lett 9: 20130037rsbl.2013.0037 [pii];10.1098/rsbl.2013.0037 []. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
57. Brosnan M, Gallop V, Iftikhar N, Keogh E (2011) Digit ratio (2D:4D), academic performance in computer science and comupter-related anxiety. Pers Individ Dif 51: 371–375
58. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD (2012) Internet and gaming addiction: a systematic literature review of neuroimaging studies. Brain Sci 2: 347–374
59. Hewig J, Kretschmer N, Trippe RH, Hecht H, Coles MG, et al. (2010) Hypersensitivity to reward in problem gamblers. Biol Psychiatry 67: 781–783S0006-3223(09)01346-8 [pii];10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.009 []. [PubMed]
60. Kim SH, Baik SH, Park CS, Kim SJ, Choi SW, et al. (2011) Reduced striatal dopamine D2 receptors in people with Internet addiction. NeuroReport 22: 407–41110.1097/WNR.0b013e328346e16e []. [PubMed]
61. Hou H, Jia S, Hu S, Fan R, Sun W, et al. (2012) Reduced striatal dopamine transporters in people with internet addiction disorder. J Biomed Biotechnol 2012: 85452410.1155/2012/854524 []. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
62. Kim KS, Kim KH (2010) [A prediction model for internet game addiction in adolescents: using a decision tree analysis]. J Korean Acad Nurs 40: 378–388201006378 [pii];10.4040/jkan.2010.40.3.378 []. [PubMed]
63. Mößle T, Rehbein F (2013) Predictors of problematic video game usage in childhood and adolescence. Sucht 59: 153–164
64. Hussain Z, Griffiths MD, Baguley T (2011) Online gaming addiction: classification, prediction and associated risk factors. Addict Res Theory 20: 1–13
65. Ko CH, Yen JY, Chen CS, Yeh YC, Yen CF (2009) Predictive values of psychiatric symptoms for internet addiction in adolescents: a 2-year prospective study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 163: 937–943163/10/937 [pii];10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.159 []. [PubMed]
66. Rehbein F, Baier D (2013) A five-year longitudinal study investigating family, media and school related risk factors of video game addiction. J Media Psychology 25: 118–128
67. Gentile DA, Choo H, Liau A, Sim T, Li D, et al. (2011) Pathological video game use among youths: a two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics 127: e319–e329peds.2010-1353 [pii];10.1542/peds.2010-1353 []. [PubMed]
68. Manning JT (2010) Digit ratio (2D:4D), sex differences, allometry, and finger length of 12-30-year olds: Evidence from the British Broadcasting Corporation (BBC) internet study. Am J Hum Biol 22: 604–60810.1002/ajhb.21051 []. [PubMed]
69. Brañas-Garza P, Kovárík J, Neyse L (2013) Second-to-fourth digit ratio has a non-monotonic impact on altruism. PLoS ONE 8: e6041910.1371/journal.pone.0060419 [];PONE-D-12-32101 [pii]. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
70. Bowman AW (2006) Comparing nonparametric surfaces. Statistical Modelling 6: 279–299
71. Bowman AW, Azzalini A (1997) Applied Smoothing Techniques for Data Analysis: the Kernel Approach with S-Plus Illustrations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
72. Kerridge BT, Saha TD, Gmel G, Rehm J (2013) Taxometric analysis of DSM-IV and DSM-5 alcohol use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend 129: 60–69S0376-8716(12)00374-2 [pii];10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.09.010 []. [PubMed]