Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Sep 13;108(37):15037-42. Epub 2011 Mar 14.
Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Tomasi D, Telang F.
Abstract
Dopamine (DA) is considered crucial for the rewarding effects of drugs of abuse, but its role in addiction is much less clear. This review focuses on studies that used PET to characterize the brain DA system in addicted subjects. These studies have corroborated in humans the relevance of drug-induced fast DA increases in striatum [including nucleus accumbens (NAc)] in their rewarding effects but have unexpectedly shown that in addicted subjects, drug-induced DA increases (as well as their subjective reinforcing effects) are markedly blunted compared with controls. In contrast, addicted subjects show significant DA increases in striatum in response to drug-conditioned cues that are associated with self-reports of drug craving and appear to be of a greater magnitude than the DA responses to the drug. We postulate that the discrepancy between the expectation for the drug effects (conditioned responses) and the blunted pharmacological effects maintains drug taking in an attempt to achieve the expected reward. Also, whether tested during early or protracted withdrawal, addicted subjects show lower levels of D2 receptors in striatum (including NAc), which are associated with decreases in baseline activity in frontal brain regions implicated in salience attribution (orbitofrontal cortex) and inhibitory control (anterior cingulate gyrus), whose disruption results in compulsivity and impulsivity. These results point to an imbalance between dopaminergic circuits that underlie reward and conditioning and those that underlie executive function (emotional control and decision making), which we postulate contributes to the compulsive drug use and loss of control in addiction.
Drugs of abuse (including alcohol) are inherently rewarding, which is why they are consumed by humans or self-administered by laboratory animals (1). Only a small percentage of individuals exposed to drugs will become addicted, that is, shift from controlled drug use to compulsive drug use with loss of control over intake despite adverse consequences, however (2). Factors that determine who becomes addicted include genetic (50% of risk), developmental (risk is higher in adolescence), and environmental (e.g., drug access, stress) factors (2).
The mesolimbic dopamine (DA) pathway [DA cells in ventral tegmental area (VTA) projecting into nucleus accumbens (NAc)] seems to be crucial for drug reward (1). Other DA pathways [mesostriatal (DA cells in substantia nigra {SN} projecting into dorsal striatum) and mesocortical (DA cells in VTA projecting into frontal cortex)] are now also recognized to contribute to drug reward and addiction (1). The mode of DA cell firing also differently modulates the rewarding and conditioning effects, of drugs (predominantly phasic DA cell firing) vs. the changes in executive function that occur in addiction (predominantly tonic DA cell firing) (3, 4).
This review summarizes studies that used PET to evaluate DA’s role in drug reward and addiction. These findings show that addiction affects not only the DA reward circuit but circuits involved with conditioning/habits, motivation, and executive functions (inhibitory control, salience attribution, and decision making). Other neurotransmitters (and neuropeptides) are involved with drug reward (i.e., cannabinoids, opioids) and with the neuroadaptations from repeated drug use (i.e., glutamate, opioids, GABA, corticotropic-releasing factor). These are not discussed here (except for glutamate), but several reviews address them (5, 6).
DA and Acute Drug Reward
All drugs that can lead to addiction increase DA in NAc, which is achieved through their interaction with different molecular targets by the various drug classes (6) (Table 1). In humans, PET studies have shown that several drugs [stimulants (7, 8), nicotine (9), alcohol (10), and marijuana (11)] increase DA in dorsal and ventral striatum (where NAc is located). These studies used a radiotracer that binds to DA D2 receptors (D2Rs) but only when these are not occupied by DA (i.e., [11C]raclopride). By comparing binding after placebo and after the drug, these studies estimate the decreases in D2R availability induced by the drug, which are proportional to DA increases (12). Most studies have reported that participants who display the greatest DA increases with the drug also report the most intense “high” or “euphoria” (reviewed ref. 13).
PET studies have also shown that the speed with which a drug enters and leaves the brain (pharmacokinetic profile) is crucial for its reinforcing effects. Specifically, PET studies of brain pharmacokinetics of drugs labeled with positron emitters show that peak levels in human brain are reached within 10 min after i.v. administration and that this fast drug uptake is associated with the high (13) (Fig. 1). Indeed, for an equivalent level of cocaine reaching the brain (assessed as equivalent level of DA transporter blockade), when cocaine entered the brain rapidly (smoked and i.v. administration), it elicited a more intense high than when it entered the brain more slowly (snorted) (14). This is consistent with preclinical studies showing that the faster the drug’s entry into the brain, the stronger are its reinforcing effects (15). This probably reflects the fact that abrupt and large DA increases triggered by drugs mimic the fast and large DA increases associated with phasic DA firing that are associated in the brain with conveying information about reward and saliency (16).
Drug-induced DA increases in NAc occur in nonaddicted as well as addicted subjects, which raises the question of how they relate to addiction.
To start with, there is increasing evidence that DA’s role in reinforcement is more complex than just coding for reward per se (hedonic pleasure) and that stimuli that induce fast and large DA increases also trigger conditioned responses and elicit incentive motivation to procure them (17). Through conditioning, a neutral stimulus that is linked with the reinforcer (i.e., natural reinforcer, drug) acquires the ability by itself to increase DA in striatum (including NAc) in anticipation of the reward, and this is associated with drug seeking (17). In animals trained to expect a natural reinforcer (food) when exposed to a conditioned stimulus (CS), the DA neurons stop responding to the primary reinforcer and, instead, respond to the CS (16). The extent to which a similar process occurs in response to drugs of abuse is unclear, however, because drugs, through their pharmacological actions, can directly activate DA neurons (i.e., nicotine) or increase DA release (i.e., amphetamine) (Table 1).
To answer this, we compared DA increases induced by the stimulant drug methylphenidate (MP) between cocaine-addicted subjects and controls. Like cocaine, MP increases DA by blocking DA transporters; both drugs have a similar distribution in human brain and have similar behavioral effects when given i.v. (18). In detoxified cocaine-addicted subjects (n = 20, detoxified 3–6 wk), we showed marked attenuation of MP-induced DA increases in striatum (50% lower) and of the increases in self-reports of high, compared with non–drug-abusing controls (n = 23). Similar findings were reported after administration of i.v. amphetamine (another stimulant drug) in recently detoxified cocaine abusers (detoxified 2 wk), who also showed decreased DA release in striatum and attenuated self-reports of euphoria (19). Because a confound in these studies was the possibility that drug withdrawal accounted for the attenuated DA responses, we repeated this study in active cocaine-addicted subjects (n = 19, nondetoxified) (20). In active cocaine abusers, MP-induced DA changes did not differ from placebo and the DA changes were 80% lower than in controls (n = 24); the self-reports of high were also attenuated (Fig. 2). Marked blunting of striatal DA increases secondary to MP or to amphetamine has also been documented in detoxified alcoholics (reviewed in ref. 13). If, as is currently believed, drug-induced DA increases in NAc underlie drug reward, why would cocaine-addicted subjects, who show a marked attenuation of drug-induced DA increases, compulsively take the drug?
DA and Conditioning to Drug Cues
The explanation may arise from the process of conditioning, which is one of the initial neuroadaptations that follow exposure to drugs and involves DA phasic signaling (predominantly D1Rs) and synaptic changes in NMDA and AMPA receptors (modulated by glutamate) (21, 22). These conditioned responses are believed to underlie the intense desire for the drug (craving) and the compulsive use that occurs when addicted subjects are exposed to drug cues.
To assess if drug conditioned cues would increase DA in cocaine-addicted subjects, we tested active cocaine-addicted subjects (n = 18) when subjects watched a neutral video (nature scenes) vs. when they watched a cocaine-cue video (scenes of subjects procuring and smoking cocaine) (23). Cocaine cues significantly increased DA in dorsal striatum, and the magnitude of this increase was correlated with the subjective experience of craving (Fig. 3); similar findings were reported by another laboratory (24). Subjects with the largest cue-induced DA increases in dorsal striatum also had the highest scores on measures of addiction severity. Because the dorsal striatum is implicated in habit learning, this association is likely to reflect the strengthening of habits as chronicity of addiction progresses. This suggests that a basic disruption in addiction might be DA-triggered conditioned responses that result in habits leading to compulsive drug consumption. Inasmuch as in cocaine-addicted subjects, the DA increases triggered by conditioned cues appear to be larger than those produced by a stimulant drug, this suggests that conditioned responses may drive the DA signaling that triggers and maintains the motivation to take the drug. To the extent that the drug (even when its DA-enhancing effects are attenuated) predicts reward, the act of its administration (e.g., injection, smoking) may become a conditioned cue and, as such, may increase DA. Thus, although drugs may initially lead to DA release in striatum (signaling reward), with repeated administration and as habits develop, there appears to be a shift in the DA increases from the drug to the CS, as reported for natural reinforcers (16). Preclinical studies have revealed that glutamatergic projections from prefrontal cortex into VTA/SN and NAc mediate these conditioned responses (5).
DA and Inhibitory Control in Addiction
The capacity to inhibit prepotent responses is likely to contribute to an individual’s ability to restrain from taking drugs, and thus his or her vulnerability to addiction (25).
PET studies have shown that addicted subjects have significant reductions in D2R availability in striatum that persist months after protracted detoxification (reviewed in ref. 13). To investigate the functional significance of the striatal D2R reductions, we have assessed their relationship to baseline measures of brain glucose metabolism (marker of brain function). We have shown that reductions in striatal D2R are associated with decreased metabolism in orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior cingulate gyrus (ACC), and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (26–28) (Fig. 4). Because OFC, CG, and DLPFC are involved with salience attribution, inhibitory control/emotion regulation, and decision making, we had postulated that their improper regulation by DA in addicted subjects could underlie the enhanced motivational value of drugs in their behavior and loss of control over drug intake (29). In addition, because impairments in OFC and ACC are associated with compulsive behaviors and impulsivity, we postulated that DA’s impaired modulation of these regions could underlie the compulsive and impulsive drug intake seen in addiction (30, 31). Indeed, in methamphetamine abusers, low striatal D2R was associated with impulsivity (32), and low striatal D2R was associated with impulsivity and predicted compulsive cocaine administration in rodents (33).
It is also possible that the initial vulnerability for drug use occurs in prefrontal regions and that repeated drug use triggers the decreases in striatal D2R. Indeed, in a study done in subjects who, despite having a high risk for alcoholism (positive family history of alcoholism), were not alcoholics, we showed higher than normal striatal D2R availability that was associated with normal metabolism in OFC, ACC, and DLPFC (25). We interpreted this to suggest that normal prefrontal function may have protected these subjects from alcohol abuse.
DA and Motivation in Addiction
DA is also involved in motivation (i.e., vigor, persistence, effort toward the pursuit of reinforcing stimuli) through its regulation of several target regions, including NAc, ACC, OFC, DLPFC, amygdala, dorsal striatum, and ventral pallidum (34).
The enhanced motivation to procure drugs is a hallmark of addiction. Drug-addicted individuals will go to extreme behaviors to obtain drugs, even at the expense of seriously adverse consequences (2). Drug seeking and drug taking become their main motivational drives, which displace other activities (35). Thus, the addicted subject is aroused and motivated when seeking to procure the drug but tends to be withdrawn and apathetic when exposed to non–drug-related activities. This shift has been studied by comparing the brain activation patterns occurring with exposure to conditioned cues with those occurring without such cues.
In contrast to the decreases in prefrontal activity reported in detoxified cocaine abusers when not stimulated with drug or drug cues (reviewed in ref. 13), these prefrontal regions become activated when cocaine abusers are exposed to craving-inducing stimuli (either drugs or cues) (36–39). Similarly, cocaine abusers studied shortly after an episode of cocaine binging showed increased metabolic activity in OFC and ACC (also dorsal striatum) that was associated with craving (40).
Moreover, when we compared the response to i.v. MP between cocaine-addicted and nonaddicted subjects, we showed that MP increased metabolism in ventral ACC and medial OFC (an effect associated with craving) only in addicted subjects, whereas it decreased metabolism in these regions in nonaddicted subjects (41). This suggests that the activation of these prefrontal regions with drug exposure may be specific to addiction and associated with the enhanced desire for the drug. Moreover, in a subsequent study in which we prompted cocaine-addicted subjects to inhibit craving purposefully when exposed to drug cues, we showed that subjects who were successful in inhibiting craving decreased metabolism in medial OFC (processes motivational value of reinforcer) and NAc (predicts reward) (42).
These findings corroborate the involvement of OFC, ACC, and striatum in the enhanced motivation to procure the drug in addiction.
Systems Model of Addiction
As summarized above, several brain circuits are relevant in the neurobiology of addiction. Here, we highlighted four of these circuits: reward/saliency, motivation/drive, conditioning/habits, and inhibitory control/executive function (Fig. 5). The mood regulation circuit (contributes to regulation of stress reactivity) and the interoception circuit (contributes to awareness of drug craving and mood) also participate in addiction, but their involvement in the human brain has been much less investigated. Consequences of the disruption of these circuits are an enhanced motivational value of the drug (secondary to learned associations through conditioning and habits) at the expense of other reinforcers (secondary to decreased sensitivity of the reward circuit) and an impaired ability to inhibit the intentional actions associated with the strong desire to take the drug (secondary to impaired executive function) that result in compulsive drug taking in addiction (35).
Although it is likely that DA changes underlie some aberrant behaviors in addiction, it is also possible that some DA changes may reflect attempts to compensate for deficits in other neurotransmitters, particularly because DA is modulated by glutamate (GABA has been less investigated). Corticostriatal glutamatergic terminals are responsible for learning well-established behaviors and for changing these behaviors when they are no longer adaptive, and neuroadaptations in these projections (and in amygdalostriatal glutamate pathways) with repeated drug use (including impaired regulation of glutamate synaptic release) are implicated in the enhanced motivation for drug seeking that occurs in addiction (5). Impairments in glutamate-induced neuroplasticity with chronic drug exposure are also likely to be involved in the prefrontal function deficits reported in addicted individuals that result in impairments in inhibitory control and in inability to change maladaptive behaviors and to learn from the adverse consequences of drug use.
This model suggests a multipronged therapeutical approach to addiction to decrease the reinforcing properties of drugs, enhance the rewarding properties of natural reinforcers, inhibit conditioned-learned associations, enhance motivation for non–drug-related activities, and strengthen inhibitory control.
Footnotes
- 1To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected].
Author contributions: N.D.V., G.-J.W., and J.S.F. designed research; N.D.V., G.-J.W., J.S.F., D.T., and F.T. performed research; N.D.V., G.-J.W., J.S.F., D.T., and F.T. analyzed data; and N.D.V. wrote the paper.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article arises from the Sackler Lecture, “Addiction: Conflict Between Brain Circuits,” presented by Nora Volkow on June 11 at the AAAS Auditorium in Washington, DC. The lecture opened the Arthur M. Sackler Colloquium of the National Academy of Sciences, on “Quantification of Behavior.” The complete program and audio files of most presentations are available on the NAS Web site at www.nasonline.org/quantification. See all papers from this colloquium in supplement 3 of volume 108.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
References