Developmental imaging genetics: linking dopamine function to adolescent behavior (2014)

Brain Cogn. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 Aug 1.

Published in final edited form as:

Brain Cogn. 2014 Aug; 89: 27–38.

Published online 2013 Oct 17. doi:  10.1016/j.bandc.2013.09.011

PMCID: PMC4226044

NIHMSID: NIHMS535184

 

Aarthi Padmanabhan1 and Beatriz Luna1

Author information ► Copyright and License information ►

The publisher’s final edited version of this article is available at Brain Cogn

See other articles in PMC that cite the published article.

Go to:

Abstract

Adolescence is a period of development characterized by numerous neurobiological changes that significantly influence behavior and brain function. Adolescence is of particular interest due to the alarming statistics indicating that mortality rates increase two to three-fold during this time compared to childhood, due largely to a peak in risk-taking behaviors resulting from increased impulsivity and sensation seeking. Furthermore, there exists large unexplained variability in these behaviors that are in part mediated by biological factors. Recent advances in molecular genetics and functional neuroimaging have provided a unique and exciting opportunity to noninvasively study the influence of genetic factors on brain function in humans. While genes do not code for specific behaviors, they do determine the structure and function of proteins that are essential to the neuronal processes that underlie behavior. Therefore, studying the interaction of genotype with measures of brain function over development could shed light on critical time points when biologically mediated individual differences in complex behaviors emerge. Here we review animal and human literature examining the neurobiological basis of adolescent development related to dopamine neurotransmission. Dopamine is of critical importance because of (1) its role in cognitive and affective behaviors, (2) its role in the pathogenesis of major psychopathology, and (3) the protracted development of dopamine signaling pathways over adolescence. We will then focus on current research examining the role of dopamine-related genes on brain function. We propose the use of imaging genetics to examine the influence of genetically mediated dopamine variability on brain function during adolescence, keeping in mind the limitations of this approach.

Go to:

Introduction

In the human lifespan, the adolescent period roughly coincides with the onset of puberty, when key neuroendocrine processes trigger and co-occur with a complex series of biological changes including, significant physical, sexual, neurochemical, neurofunctional, physiological, cardiovascular, and respiratory maturation (Falkner and Tanner 1986; Romeo 2003). These biological changes reciprocally interact with the environment and characterize a vulnerable and dynamic period of physical, psychological, and social development (Spear, 2000). Across species and cultures, there are characteristic behaviors during adolescence including peaks in sensation/novelty seeking coupled with diminished levels of harm avoidance, leading to an increase in risky behaviors (Laviola, Macri et al. 2003). Normative increases in sensation/novelty seeking can be adaptive, allowing adolescents to seek independence outside of the home. In other words, some risks might be necessary to facilitate the transition into adult roles in society. However, certain behaviors that have high subjective desirability can also expose an individual to harmful consequences (Spear, 2000). Thus, we define risk-taking as engaging in a behavior with potential rewarding outcomes (also known as incentive-driven behavior), but high potential negative consequences. The consequences of risky behaviors that peak in adolescence (e.g. experimentation with drugs and alcohol, reckless driving, and unprotected sex), can be dramatic as mortality and morbidity rates increase significantly from childhood (Dahl 2004). In addition to the risks of normative development, adolescence is often a time when various mental illnesses emerge such as mood disorders, drug abuse disorders, eating disorders, and psychoses (Pine 2002; Chambers, Taylor et al. 2003; Sisk and Zehr 2005; Paus, Keshavan et al. 2008), the risk factors for which are not fully characterized. In light of this evidence, it is also important to note adolescents are capable of mature decision-making (Paus 2005), abstract thinking, and often engage rational behavior (Steinberg, Cauffman et al. 2009). Thus, many of the classic risk-taking behaviors observed in adolescence are often in the context of highly emotive and/or reward-seeking states (Casey, Getz et al. 2008; Blakemore and Robbins 2012), highlighting a unique and universal biological vulnerability and neuroplasticity that is not fully characterized.

Despite evidence of overall increases in risk taking behaviors in adolescence, with the assumption that each individual is at their own peak in sensation and novelty seeking, there is much variability in adolescent behavior that remains unexplained. That is, while some adolescents are high risk-takers, others are not, and the contexts under which certain individuals engage in risk-taking vary. In recent years, the field of genetics has merged with cognitive neuroscience to examine the neurobiological basis of variability in behavior. This approach, known as ‘imaging genetics’, is grounded in the idea that brain function and structure can serve as intermediate phenotypes between genes and behavior, given the relative proximity of brain function to the genotype (Hariri and Weinberger 2003).

This review focuses on the influence of the neurotransmitter dopamine and variations in dopamine genes on incentive-driven behaviors in adolescence. We first review the literature on the maturation of key brain systems – namely frontostriatal circuits – and their role in adolescent behavior. The role of dopamine in modulating motivated behaviors and the protracted development of dopamine function through adolescence will be discussed next. Lastly, we focus on a review of imaging genetics studies using common functional polymorphisms in key dopamine signaling genes leading to a proposal for future research in adolescent brain development.

Go to:

Incentive driven behaviors and frontostriatal circuits in adolescence

Evidence suggests that adolescents tend to both process incentives differently than adults (for reviews see: Geier and Luna (2009; Ernst, Daniele et al. 2011)), leading to suboptimal and often risky decision-making. The framework of adolescent incentive processing is contingent on the idea that adolescents are biased towards potential rewards (Steinberg 2004) and display immature cognitive control (Yurgelun-Todd 2007), with continued maturation in the brain systems that underlie both (Casey, Getz et al. 2008; Ernst and Fudge 2009).

The human striatum is recognized as a core node for incentive processing and resulting behaviors, specifically in the ability to synthesize changing environmental cues and appropriately update behaviors through integration with the prefrontal cortex (PFC) by way of overlapping but functionally segregated pathways (Alexander, DeLong et al. 1986; Postuma and Dagher 2006; Di Martino, Scheres et al. 2008) that are underlie distinct behaviors (Tekin and Cummings 2002). Major frontal-striatal circuits function by way of excitatory projections from frontal regions to specific striatal areas (e.g. dorsolateral PFC to dorsal caudate, lateral OFC to ventromedial caudate, medial OFC to nucleus accumbens) and back via the thalamus. These closed-loop circuits result in two major pathways; direct and indirect. The direct pathway, which disinhibits the thalamus, involves GABAergic projections from striatum to midbrain to the internal segment of the globus pallidus to the thalamus. The indirect pathway consists of GABAergic projections from striatum to the globus pallidus externa to the subthalamic nucleus, finally exciting inhibitory neurons in the globus pallidus interna, which inhibit the thalamus. Thus, favored behaviors are activated via the direct pathway and the indirect pathway inhibits less desirable and competing actions. Thus, immaturities and disturbances in the function of frontostriatal circuits may result in competition between the direct and indirect pathways, leading to suboptimal behaviors.

To this end, neurobiological models of adolescent development suggest that an over active adolescent incentive system, driven by the striatum, with a still maturing cognitive system, driven by the PFC, may create a functional imbalance in optimal behavioral regulation (i.e. suppressing a potentially rewarding, but inappropriate behavior) thereby enhancing risk taking behavior in adolescence ((Nelson, Leibenluft et al. 2005; Ernst, Pine et al. 2006; Casey, Getz et al. 2008), for a summary of these models see Sturman and Moghaddam, (2011)). Indeed, functional neuroimaging studies of incentive processing demonstrate differential striatal and PFC activation in adolescence relative to adulthood (Bjork, Knutson et al. 2004; Ernst, Nelson et al. 2005; Galvan, Hare et al. 2006; Bjork, Smith et al. 2010; van Leijenhorst, Moor et al. 2010; Padmanabhan 2011), with the majority of studies reporting an increase in striatal activation, coupled with decreases in prefrontal recruitment. Furthermore, functional connectivity studies suggest that the integration and coordination between brain regions, including subcortical to cortical connections, become more refined and efficient over adolescence, leading to reduced task-irrelevant connections, strengthening of connections supporting goal-directed actions, and elimination of redundant connections (Durston, Davidson et al. 2006; Liston, Watts et al. 2006; Fair, Cohen et al. 2009; Stevens, Pearlson et al. 2009; Hwang, Velanova et al. 2010). Animal and post-mortem human literature suggests an overexpression of receptors for serotonin, dopamine, adenergic, and endocannabinoids (Lidow and Rakic 1992), a peak in the density of interneurons (Anderson, Classey et al. 1995; Lewis 1997; Erickson and Lewis 2002), and an increase in levels of GABA (Hedner, Iversen et al. 1984). These changes alter the excitatory-inhibitory balance in neuronal signaling that refine controlled processing into adulthood. Lastly, increased myelination in cortical to subcortical axons, changes in axon caliber, pruning of synapses and receptors, cell shrinkage, and glial changes (Yakovlev and Lecours 1967; Rakic, Bourgeois et al. 1986; Benes, Turtle et al. 1994; Andersen 2003) refine the developing brain and strengthen and consolidate highly used connections, while weakening or eliminating redundant or weakly used connections through unique experiences ((Huttenlocher 1990; Jernigan, Trauner et al. 1991; Pfefferbaum, Mathalon et al. 1994; Giedd, Blumenthal et al. 1999), for review see: (Paus 2005)). Taken together, the current literature highlights that immaturities in the function of and integration between frontal and striatal regions at multiple levels of organization contribute to a distinct adolescent brain (and subsequently behavioral) phenotype.

Go to:

Dopamine

Frontostriatal circuits subserving affective, cognitive, and motor processes are significantly modulated by the neurotransmitter dopamine (DA) (for reviews see (Schultz 2002; Wise 2004; Cools 2008), through facilitation of the direct pathway via the action of excitatory DA receptors (D1-like) and inhibition of the indirect pathway via the action of inhibitory DA receptors (D2-like). DA neurons in the midbrain project to medium spiny neurons in the NAcc as well as pyramidal neurons in the PFC, thereby modulating the firing rates of these neurons and establishing a strong reciprocal relationship between striatum and PFC (Grace, Floresco et al. 2007). DA levels are modulated by two dissociable processes of DA discharge that interact, (1) a constant background tonicity regulated by baseline firing of DA neurons and glutamatergic afferents from cortical to striatal regions, and (2) a burst firing high-amplitude phasic release (Grace, Floresco et al. 2007). These two mechanisms of DA signaling have been found to lead to distinct behaviors (Floresco, West et al. 2003) and are regulated by reuptake and degradation enzymes. Fast phasic events occur in response to reward-related events, which may serve as important teaching signals for error detection and modulate behavioral changes in response to the environment (Schultz 1998). Slow changes in tonic levels of DA may be a preparatory mechanism for an organism to respond to environmental cues associated with reward (Schultz 1998). These systems also interact as tonic DA activity regulates phasic signaling in an inhibitory fashion and phasic DA has been shown to enhance tonic activity (Niv, Daw et al. 2007).

The DA system undergoes significant change over adolescence, which is relevant for adolescent behavior for several reasons. First, DA signaling supports reinforcement learning as it tunes the strength of synapses, thereby influencing plasticity. Second, DA modulation of striatal and prefrontal function influences affective and motivated behaviors that are altered in adolescence. Lastly, abnormalities in DA signaling are implicated in the pathophysiology of neuropsychiatric disorders that often emerge in adolescence (e.g. schizophrenia, drug abuse). The literature spanning the development of DA function and implications for adolescent behavior has been reviewed in depth elsewhere, (Spear 2000; Chambers, Taylor et al. 2003; O’Donnell 2010; Wahlstrom, Collins et al. 2010; Wahlstrom, White et al. 2010; Luciana, Wahlstrom et al. 2012) and is summarized below. Much of the evidence on the DA system in adolescence is from non-human primate and rodent models and findings are not straightforward. With this caveat in mind, the relevant literature is briefly summarized below to highlight an overall trend that may have implications for adolescent behavior.

A peak in activity of midbrain DA neurons has been documented in the rat model (McCutcheon, White et al. 2009), suggesting an overall increase in DA levels. Other studies have noted a peak in tonic DA concentrations in late adolescence with a subsequent decline in adulthood ((Badanich, Adler et al. 2006; Philpot, Wecker et al. 2009). Non-human primate studies show that the highest concentrations of DA during adolescence are in the PFC before dropping down in adulthood (Goldman-Rakic and Brown 1982). In human post-mortem studies, DA levels in the striatum increase until adolescence and then decrease or remain the same (Haycock, Becker et al. 2003). In one study, extracellular levels of DA in the NAcc were lower in adolescence compared to adulthood (Cao, Lotfipour et al. 2007). Dopaminergic innervation to the PFC peaks in adolescence (Rosenberg and Lewis 1995; Benes, Taylor et al. 2000), with the largest increase being in cortical layer III, a region that that is highly implicated in cognitive processing (Lewis and Gonzalez-Burgos 2000). These changes occur both in length of individual axons and as well as total number of projecting axons (Rosenberg and Lewis 1994; Lambe, Krimer et al. 2000). There is also an increase in the density of synapses between DA neurons and pyramidal neurons in layer III of cortex (Lambe, Krimer et al. 2000) as well as a peak in glutamatergic connectivity from the PFC to the NAcc, specifically in D1-expressing neurons (Brenhouse, Sonntag et al. 2008). Regarding receptor densities, non-human primate research suggests that the density of D1 and D2 receptors in PFC increase at different rates, with D1 receptor density demonstrating earlier peaks than D2, which peaks in late-adolescence/early adulthood (Tseng and O’Donnell 2007). A post mortem human research study found that D1 receptor densities peak around 14–18 years of age (Weickert, Webster et al. 2007), declining thereafter. A peak in cells containing D1 receptors in the PFC has also been documented (Andersen, Thompson et al. 2000; Weickert, Webster et al. 2007). In the striatum, peaks in both D1 and D2 receptors occur in childhood and begin to decline in adolescence, evident in both animal and human work (Seeman, Bzowej et al. 1987; Lidow and Rakic 1992; Montague, Lawler et al. 1999; Andersen, Thompson et al. 2002). However, other evidence suggests that DA receptor densities decline in dorsal, but not ventral striatum (where levels remain the same) over adolescence (Teicher, Andersen et al. 1995). Research on DA transporters has been inconsistent in the midbrain suggesting no consistent developmental change (Moll, Mehnert et al. 2000), increases over adolescence (Galineau, Kodas et al. 2004), and peaks in late childhood (Coulter, Happe et al. 1996). Other research has suggested that in the striatum, DA transporter levels increase into late childhood and remain stable through adolescence (Coulter, Happe et al. 1996; Tarazi, Tomasini et al. 1998; Galineau, Kodas et al. 2004).

Adding to this complexity, maturational changes in DA function have not been mapped directly onto behaviors in adolescence suggesting that a comprehensive examination of the interaction of various aspects of the DA system (e.g. receptors, clearance, innervation) and their direct effects on behavior is warranted (Spear 2011; Luciana, Wahlstrom et al. 2012). For example, the elevation of tonic DA during adolescence may impact regulation of the phasic response in response to salient or rewarding information (for review see (Luciana, Wahlstrom et al. 2012)), but this has not been empirically tested. It is posited that the DA system is at a “functional ceiling” in adolescence relative to childhood or adulthood (Chambers, Taylor et al. 2003), due to peaks in midbrain DA cell firing, overall tonic levels, innervation, as well as increased receptor densities. The adult literature suggests that increasing DA signaling through administration of DA or DA agonists increases novelty-seeking and exploration behaviors, whereas reducing DA signaling with antagonists halts such behaviors (Pijnenburg, Honig et al. 1976; Fouriezos, Hansson et al. 1978; Le Moal and Simon 1991). These early findings point to a hypothesized model of adolescent DA function whereby overall heightened DA signaling leads to heightened motivation, or approach-like behaviors-due to increased activation of the direct pathway and inhibition of the indirect pathway. Other evidence associating altered DA in adolescence to behavior suggest that adolescent rodents exhibit increased reinforcing effects to drugs that influence DA release, such as alcohol, nicotine, amphetamines, and cocaine (Adriani, Chiarotti et al. 1998; Laviola, Adriani et al. 1999; Adriani and Laviola 2000; Badanich, Adler et al. 2006; Shram, Funk et al. 2006; Frantz, O’Dell et al. 2007; Mathews and McCormick 2007; Brenhouse and Andersen 2008; Varlinskaya and Spear 2010). Adolescents also show decreased aversive response to substances of abuse (i.e. milder withdrawal responses, reduced psychomotor effects) (Spear 2002; Doremus, Brunell et al. 2003; Levin, Rezvani et al. 2003) and increased sensitivity to DA receptor antagonists (Spear, Shalaby et al. 1980; Spear and Brake 1983; Teicher, Barber et al. 1993). Research in adult human and animal models has suggested that intermediate levels of DA signaling in both PFC and striatum are necessary for optimal performance, following a Yerkes-Dodson inverted U-shaped dose response curve of DA signaling and behavior (Robbins and Arnsten 2009; Cools and D’Esposito 2011). Following this model, increased DA levels in adolescence may surpass the threshold required for optimal functioning (Wahlstrom, Collins et al. 2010; Wahlstrom, White et al. 2010). DA signaling in adolescence may also influence and be influenced by differences in rates of maturation of subcortical systems relative to cortical, and an functional imbalance in the adolescent brain that is driven by striatal signaling with immaturities in PFC-driven regulation (Chambers, Taylor et al. 2003; Ernst, Pine et al. 2006).

Despite an overall peak in DA signaling and general maturational processes that occur in adolescence, there is considerable individual variability both in DA signaling, as well as DA-influenced behaviors, likely due to a combination of genetic and environmental factors (Depue and Collins 1999; Frank and Hutchison 2009). Understanding the nature of these individual differences may have significant predictive power. For example, adolescents with higher levels of tonic DA levels, higher DA receptor densities, and lower rates of DA clearance and degradation may engage in DA-modulated behaviors (e.g. sensation/novelty seeking) to a larger extent than adolescents with decreased DA signaling and availability (For review see (Luciana, Wahlstrom et al. 2012)). These hypothesized patterns are based on prior adult studies that highlight the importance of the baseline state of the DA system – which varies across individuals. For example, increasing DA levels in individuals who have high baseline DA levels impairs cognitive performance, (perhaps pushing them over the peak of the inverted U curve) whereas improvements are noted in individuals with lower baseline levels (pushing them closer to the apex of the curve) (Mattay, Goldberg et al. 2003; Apud, Mattay et al. 2007; Cools, Frank et al. 2009). While this model is simplistic, we use this as a framework to study the genetic factors that drive variability in DA function, and how these factors may interact with normative changes over development. Following this model, it is possible that baseline inter-individual differences in adolescence would be unique relative to differences in adulthood due to maturation in the DA system.

Go to:

Developmental Imaging Genetics

Methodologically, characterizing the nature of neurochemical systems in human development is challenging, as pharmacological and other invasive procedures (i.e. PET) typically cannot be used to study developing populations. In an effort to develop biologically plausible and testable hypotheses on the influence of DA on brain function, recent efforts have focused on identifying variants in the human genome that directly impact protein function and subsequently cellular and systems-level brain function. Researchers have used functional and structural neuroimaging measures as intermediate phenotypes to better understand the influence of genetic variability on human behavior (Hariri and Weinberger 2003). This approach is grounded in the notion that genetic influences on behavior are mediated by changes in cellular and systems levels of functioning in the brain. Indeed, the study of the influence of genetic polymorphisms on brain function or “imaging genetics” has already provided considerable insight on the influence of genetically driven variability on brain physiology (e.g. (Hariri and Weinberger 2003; Brown and Hariri 2006; Drabant, Hariri et al. 2006; Hariri and Lewis 2006)). However see: (Flint and Munafo 2007; Walters and Owen 2007; Kendler and Neale 2010) for limitations and considerations of this approach. The rationale for imaging genetics studies is that, with its incisive methodological tools and its capacity for deriving detailed structural and functional information, brain imaging holds particular promise for linking the effects of genes on behavior. Given that the development of the DA system may affect some individuals more than others and that genetic effects are likely not static, and change across the lifespan, studying the influence of genetically-driven variability of the DA system on brain development has great potential to elucidate the biological basis of individual differences in behavior as well as risk for developing psychopathology.

Variants in genes that code for various DA-related proteins have previously been associated with inter-individual differences in frontostriatal brain function and structure (e.g. (Bertolino, Blasi et al. 2006; Drabant, Hariri et al. 2006; Yacubian, Sommer et al. 2007; Dreher, Kohn et al. 2009; Aarts, Roelofs et al. 2010), and with variability in behavioral phenotypes that are relevant to the study of adolescence including impulsivity, novelty seeking, aggressive traits, executive function, incentive processing, drug abuse, and the etiology of neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia, ADHD and Parkinson’s disease ((Karayiorgou, Altemus et al. 1997; Eley, Lichtenstein et al. 2003; Enoch, Schuckit et al. 2003; Lee, Lahey et al. 2007), for review see (Nemoda, Szekely et al. 2011)). In the following sections we review neuroimaging studies of common functional polymorphisms in genes that influence DA signaling. We will discuss studies of both single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNP) and variable nucleotide tandem repeat (VNTR) polymorphisms. We focus specifically on imaging genetics studies using functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI and fMRI). As evidence of behavioral associations with DA-related genes have been reviewed in depth elsewhere (e.g. (Nemoda, Szekely et al. 2011; Cormier, Muellner et al. 2013), we focus solely on imaging genetics research. Although this review is focused on normative development, we have summarized main findings of developmental imaging genetics research in both typical development and developmental disorders involving DA (such as schizophrenia and ADHD) in Table 1.

Table thumbnail

Go to:

DA Receptor Genes (DRD1, DRD2, and DRD4)

The distribution of both D1-(D1 and D5) and D2 (D2, D3, D4) -like receptors across the brain results in a complex balance of excitatory-inhibitory neuronal signaling, which exerts a strong influence on frontostriatal function and connectivity, with the largest density of receptors being in the striatum. Both D1 and D2-like receptors are G protein-coupled, and serve opposing roles, increasing and inhibiting cyclic adenosine monophosphate respectively, thereby exciting or inhibiting the activity of the neuron. D1 and D2 receptors thus have complementary roles. D1 receptors stimulation allows for maintenance of information online and stabilization of functional states, and D2 receptor binding is involved in flexible updating of information and allowing for the transition between functional states (Seamans, Durstewitz et al. 2001; Durstewitz and Seamans 2002; Seamans and Yang 2004). D1 receptors are more abundant in the direct pathway, exciting GABAergic neurons in response to preferred behaviors, and D2 in the indirect pathway, which inhibit GABAergic neurons and reduce the inhibitory effect of the indirect pathway. Increases in both D1 and D2 receptors, as seen in adolescence thus may have an overall excitatory effect on the brain, which could result in an increase in behaviors that are DA dependent (such as reward and novelty seeking).

In the PFC, D1 receptors act on glutamatergic pyramidal cells, increasing task related firing (Farde, Halldin et al. 1987; Goldman-Rakic 1990; Lidow, Goldman-Rakic et al. 1991). Simultaneously, D1 receptor activation on local GABAergic (inhibitory) interneurons serves to inhibit irrelevant glutamatergic inputs (Durstewitz, Seamans et al. 2000). Limited research has examined polymorphisms of the D1-receptor gene (DRD1) in relation to brain structure/function. One study using adults demonstrated altered prefrontal-parietal functional connectivity during a working memory task in schizophrenic patients genotyped for the DRD1 Dde I single nucleotide polymorphism consisting of an A to G substitution in the 50 UTR (Tura, Turner et al. 2008). AG heterozygotes, who have increased D1 receptors, showed increased recruitment of DLPFC relative to AA homozygotes, who engaged a more widely distributed set of brain regions. These findings are in line with other work suggesting that increased prefrontal DA tone results in improved cognitive performance and more efficient prefrontal signaling (e.g. (Egan, Goldberg et al. 2001; Mattay, Goldberg et al. 2003)).

The D2 receptor, which is expressed more abundantly in striatum relative to PFC, exerts a strong influence on frontostriatal connectivity through both inhibition of excitatory and disinhib tion of inhibitory pathways (Cepeda and Levine 1998; Goto and Grace 2005). D2 receptors have two distinct isoforms, the short isoform (D2-S) acts mainly as a presynaptic autoreceptor, inhibiting DA release, whereas the long isoform (D2-L) primarily functions to inhibit the post synaptic cell (Centonze, Grande et al. 2003). Decreased D2 autoreceptor function increases DA release and individuals with decreased D2-S demonstrate increased novelty-seeking and reward reactivity (Zald, Cowan et al. 2008; Pecina, Mickey et al. 2012). Functional polymorphisms in the gene that codes for the D2 receptor (DRD2) that influence mRNA transcription of the protein, and ultimately its function have been identified including, −141 C Ins/Del, Ser311Cys, Taq1A ANKK1, Taq1B, C957T, rs12364283, rs2283265 and rs1076560 (Zhang, Bertolino et al. 2007). Polymorphisms that influence D2 binding include the DRD2/ANNK1 TaqIA, a restriction fragment length polymorphism that results in a Glu to Lys amino acid substitution in the neighboring ANNK1 gene, and the −141C Ins/Del SNP that is located in the promotor region of the DRD2 gene. The TaqI A1 allele and the Del allele have been associated with decreased striatal D2 binding (Arinami, Gao et al. 1997; Noble 2000), although one study suggests molecular heterosis with the TaqIA polymorphism, with decreased D2 density in heterozygotes relative to homozygotes (Pohjalainen, Nagren et al. 1999). Thus, the Del and A1 alleles have been associated with increased reward reactivity in ventral striatum in adulthood (Cohen, Young et al. 2005; Forbes, Brown et al. 2009). The A1 allele has also been associated with decreased prefrontal activation and connectivity in frontostriatal circuits during task switching (Stelzel, Basten et al. 2010).

In contrast to the adult research, the few studies using only adolescent participants found that the A1 allele is associated with decreased reward reactivity in ventral (Stice and Dagher 2010) and dorsal (Stice, Spoor et al. 2008) striatum. In adolescence, when there is a higher density of D2 receptors, the relationship between brain activation and D2 receptor availability might parallel previous findings using pharmacological interventions that target D2 receptors (Kirsch, Reuter et al. 2006; van der Schaaf, van Schouwenburg et al. 2012), suggesting an age by genotype interaction that is yet to be empirically tested.

The D4 receptor is D2-like and is expressed on both postsynaptic striatal neurons and pre-synaptic corticostriatal glutamatergic afferents. Limited evidence suggests that D4 receptors develop similarly to D2 receptors (with peaks in late childhood and subsequent declines into adulthood) (Tarazi, Tomasini et al. 1998). The gene (DRD4) that codes for the D4 receptor has several functional polymorphisms, of which the 48-base pair VNTR in exon 3 that results most commonly in a 7-repeat or 4-repeat variant, is frequently studied. The 7-repeat allele is associated with decreased postsynaptic inhibition of DA, due to reduced cAMP-reduction potency, leading to a disinhibition of striatal neurons (Asghari, Sanyal et al. 1995; Seeger, Schloss et al. 2001), and has been associated with increased reward related reactivity in ventral striatum, relative to the 4-repeat allele (Schoots and Van Tol 2003; Forbes, Brown et al. 2009; Stice, Yokum et al. 2012). A SNP in the DRD4 gene (rs6277, −521 SNP) results in a 40% reduction in RNA transcription for the T-allele relative to the C-allele (Okuyama, Ishiguro et al. 1999), although another study found no differences (Kereszturi, Kiraly et al. 2006). To date, one imaging study has reported that individuals homozygous for the C allele exhibit increased medial PFC/anterior cingulate activation during the processing of reward magnitude (Camara, Kramer et al. 2010). Only the DRD4 VNTR has been studied in developing populations, associating the 7-repeat allele reduced cortical thickness in the PFC of children (Shaw, Gornick et al. 2007), increased striatal activation to incentives in children and adolescents as a moderator of anxiety in adolescents (Perez-Edgar, Hardee et al. 2013), and decreased activation to food rewards as a moderator of weight gain in adolescents (Stice, Yokum et al. 2010). The effects of this polymorphism on brain function in adolescence thus may parallel the adult findings.

Collectively, these studies demonstrate that functional variants in DA receptor genes influence frontostriatal brain function in children, adolescents and adults separately. However, no studies to date have examined the influence of these polymorphisms across development. Current research suggests that D1 and D2 receptor densities peak in late childhood, suggesting that receptor density is higher in adolescence relative to adulthood. Following the inverted U model, increased D1 and D2 receptor availability may result in increased competition between the direct and indirect pathways which may be more exacerbated in adolescents with higher receptor availability at baseline, leading to a generally more disorganized processing system.

Go to:

DA inactivation genes (COMT, DAT1)

Functional Polymorphism in the COMT Gene

Catechol-O methyltransferase (COMT), an enzyme for catecholamine catabolism, is vital to regulating DA turnover in the PFC where DA transporters are scarce (Hong, Shu-Leong et al. 1998; Matsumoto, Weickert et al. 2003). Within the COMT gene (COMT) is a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) resulting in a methionine (met) to valine (val) substitution at codon 158 (Tunbridge 2010). The COMT val allele is associated with high enzymatic activity and consequently low synaptic dopamine levels, whereas the COMT met allele results in approximately one third less enzyme activity and consequently high synaptic dopamine (Chen, Lipska et al. 2004). Heterozygotes show intermediate levels of COMT activity. Despite being predominantly expressed in the PFC, the COMT val158met polymorphism is also associated with downstream effects on midbrain DA activity (Meyer-Lindenberg, Kohn et al. 2005).

The COMT val158met SNP has been widely studied in the context of frontostriatal activation during cognitive tasks (Egan, Goldberg et al. 2001; Bilder, Volavka et al. 2002; Malhotra, Kestler et al. 2002; Goldberg, Egan et al. 2003; Mattay, Goldberg et al. 2003; Diamond, Briand et al. 2004) including working memory, response inhibition, set shifting and reward processing. Evidence suggests that individuals with the met allele demonstrate more efficient cortical function (e.g.(Egan, Goldberg et al. 2001; Mattay, Goldberg et al. 2003; Meyer-Lindenberg, Kohn et al. 2005)) as well as reward-related increases in striatal activation (Yacubian, Sommer et al. 2007; Dreher, Kohn et al. 2009) relative to individuals with the val allele. Furthermore, increasing DA levels interacts with the COMT val158met SNP consistent with the putative inverted U model with met individuals demonstrating diminished cortical efficiency during tasks of cognitive control and val individuals demonstrating improvements (Mattay, Goldberg et al. 2003; Apud, Mattay et al. 2007). Based on this evidence, it is posited that adolescents, who have increased DA levels relative to adults, may follow a similar pattern as a function of COMT genotype as the pharmacological studies in adults. This is adolescents carrying the met allele may surpass optimal thresholds, which could result in less efficient cortical function, relative to val (Wahlstrom, Collins et al. 2010; Wahlstrom, White et al. 2010). It is thus possible that inter-individual differences are expressed differentially as a function of relative DA across development based on genotype (e.g. the val allele may confer a relative advantage for cognitive function earlier in development, when DA levels are higher than in adulthood). However, limited research has examined the influence of the COMT val158met polymorphism in the adolescent brain, and these initial studies are mixed and require replication. During a visuo-spatial working memory task in individuals between the ages of 6 and 20,Dumontheil et al. (2011), demonstrated that activation in frontal and parietal regions increased across development in individuals homozygous for the val allele, but not met carriers, suggesting delayed development of cognitive function in individuals with the val allele. Val/val homozygotes also showed slower cortical thinning over development in posterior parietal cortex, perhaps reflecting slower pruning and relative inefficiency in cortical processing. COMT effects in adolescence have also been found in studies of structural and functional connectivity, with adolescents with the val allele showing increased white matter integrity and decreased resting brain perfusion relative to met (Thomason, Waugh et al. 2009; Thomason, Dougherty et al. 2010), although these studies weren’t developmental with no adult comparison groups. Lastly, one lifespan study (ranging from 6–84 years) showed reduced gray matter volume in ventral PFC in met/met individuals relative to val/val but no age by genotype interactions (Williams, Gatt et al. 2008).

Functional Polymorphism in the DAT1 Gene

The DA transporter (DAT) is mainly expressed in the striatum and is responsible for DA reuptake, clearing DA from the extracellular space after release (Jaber, Bloch et al. 1998). A VNTR polymorphism in the gene that codes for DAT (DAT1 or SLC6A3) results in alleles between 3 and 13 repeats of a 40-base pair sequence in its 3’ untranslated region (Vandenbergh, Persico et al. 1992) as coding region variants are quite rare. The DAT binding site density for the most common repeat alleles (9-repeat and 10-repeat) is significantly less for the 9-repeat allele than the 10-repeat allele, linking the 9-repeat allele with reduced DAT expression and greater striatal synaptic DA (Fuke, Suo et al. 2001; Mill, Asherson et al. 2002; VanNess, Owens et al. 2005), although some studies have suggested the opposite (Mill, Asherson et al. 2002; van de Giessen, de Win et al. 2009). Lower DAT expression reduces synaptic DA clearance thereby increasing DA levels (Cagniard, Balsam et al. 2006; Cagniard, Beeler et al. 2006). FMRI research most consistently associates the 9R allele with increased reward reactivity in the striatum (Yacubian, Sommer et al. 2007; Dreher, Kohn et al. 2009; Forbes, Brown et al. 2009). Although DAT is primarily expressed in striatum, evidence associates the 9-repeat allele with increased ventral striatal and dorsomedial PFC activation during working memory updating and task switching (Aarts, Roelofs et al. 2010; Garcia-Garcia, Barcelo et al. 2010), and increased PFC activation during inhibitory control, which was interpreted as supporting improved inhibitory control (Congdon, Lesch et al. 2008; Congdon, Constable et al. 2009). Developmental studies using the DAT1 polymorphism suggest that typically developing adolescents with the 9-repeat allele demonstrate reduced activation of prefrontal and striatal regions during inhibitory control (Braet, Johnson et al. 2011), and reward prediction (Paloyelis, Mehta et al. 2012). These results suggest that DAT1 genotype may influence the system differentially in adolescence – with the 9-repeat allele resulting in decreased striatal and cortical reactivity-than in adulthood – when the 9-repeat allele has been associated with increased activation. It is possible that in adolescence, when excess DA levels are present, individuals carrying the 9-repeat allele have an overabundance of synaptic DA availability, which may have opposite effects on brain function than in adulthood.

Go to:

Gene-Gene Interactions

Imaging genetics research has predominantly focused on single functional polymorphisms in candidate genes. The complexity of the DA system, the differing rates of maturation of various aspects of the system, the interactions of the various components of the system, and the interaction of the DA system with other brain processes, suggests that gene effects are likely not independent or dichotomous. Investigators have more recently started to study interactions between or cumulative effects of multiple genes. Given evidence that various aspects of the DA system are heightened or changed in adolescence and that single gene effects may manifest differently in the adolescent brain, it is also possible that gene interactions differ in the adolescent brain than in the adult brain. Assuming equal effect sizes of each polymorphism, prior studies have demonstrated effects on brain activation as a function of interactions between genes (Bertolino, Blasi et al. 2006; Yacubian, Sommer et al. 2007; Bertolino, Di et al. 2008; Dreher, Kohn et al. 2009). For example, prior studies have shown additive effects of the COMT val158met SNP and the DAT1 3’VNTR during the reward anticipation and outcome stages of reward processing in both PFC and striatum, reporting increased activation associated with genotypes that have increased DA availability (Yacubian, Sommer et al. 2007; Dreher, Kohn et al. 2009). However, due to limited sample sizes, these studies have only examined two polymorphisms as once. More recently, researchers have explored the influence of several DA genes on brain function during reward processing using a “multilocus composite score” (Plomin, Haworth et al. 2009), assigning each participant a single additive score based on relative levels of DA signaling. The idea behind this approach is that combining multiple functionally relevant genes through a cumulative profile score may explain more variability than single loci that may independently have non-significant effects. This research combining COMT, DAT1, and DA receptor genotypes has shown increased ventral striatal reactivity as a function of increasing DA signaling in adulthood (Nikolova, Ferrell et al. 2011), and caudate and putamen in adolescence (Stice, Yokum et al. 2012) during receipt of monetary rewards. Replication of these findings, and exploration of gene interactions over development is necessary in order to better understand cumulative effects of genotype.

Go to:

Considerations and Future Directions for Imaging Genetics studies

The genetic basis for complex behavioral traits is likely a result of allelic variation across many genes/polymorphisms and their interactions with each other and the environment. The majority of imaging genetics research has focused on associations between brain function and single or a handful of genes or polymorphisms. In addition, because neuroimaging studies require relatively evenly distributed groups, imaging genetics research is predominantly focused on high frequency alleles that are evenly distributed in the population thus having favorable or neutral effects. The downside to this approach is that these variants only explain only a small proportion of the variance in complex disorders or traits. Therefore, the main purpose of imaging genetics is not to find causal genetic links, but to better understand the neural underpinnings of complex behaviors.

Since single genetic polymorphisms have very small effects on multidimensional and heterogeneous behaviors and traits, the study of the influence of common variants on brain function requires maximal sensitivity and reliability of the measures obtained. Imaging genetics studies should utilize well-defined and objectively measured phenotypes of interest (i.e. fMRI tasks used must reliably and robustly engage circumscribed brain systems and demonstrate variance across participants). fMRI is one the most common and reliable methods of measuring brain function at decent spatial and temporal resolutions, but given that it is an indirect measure of brain activity, reflecting a paradigm related change in metabolic consumption (Logothetis, Pauls et al. 2001), interpretation of gene effects is limited. Thus, combining multimodal approaches that measure brain function and structure at varying spatial and temporal resolutions and creating adequate measures of environmental factors would be beneficial for further understanding genetic effects on brain function (Bigos and Hariri 2007; Fisher, Munoz et al. 2008; Nemoda, Szekely et al. 2011). Genetics research would also benefit from translational work, studying the influence of candidate genes in both humans and genetically modified animal models using similar behavioral/neurofunctional phenotypes (Casey, Soliman et al. 2010). Despite the limitations of translating human behavior to animals, studies using genetically modified mouse models for key DA genes, including COMT and DA receptor genes have demonstrated similar cognitive and behavioral effects similarly to humans (for review see (Casey, Soliman et al. 2010)). Thus, it is possible that gene effects on the brain would also show important similarities across species. Furthermore, developmental animal models have the advantage of shorter lifespans and stricter control of the environment.

Another way to improve reliability in imaging research is to use sample sizes that afford the power to detect small to medium effects. Initial reports have suggested that the relative proximity of brain function to the genotype may permit gene effects to be observed in fewer participants than typical behavioral studies. For example,Munafo et al. (2008) conducted a meta-analysis of studies that have reported associations between a VNTR polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) and amygdala activation and suggested that an imaging genetics study would require a total sample of about 70 participants to achieve .8 power for an alpha power of .05. Assuming a relatively even distribution of the alleles, this would result in approximately 30–35 participants per group. Similarly, others have suggested that sample sizes of over 25 subjects in each group are necessary for general fMRI studies in order to have adequate reliability (Thirion, Pinel et al. 2007). Meta-analyses to determine effect sizes of previous imaging genetics studies and ideal sample sizes for future ones is warranted for studies of DA-gene polymorphisms (Munafo, Bowes et al. 2005; Barnett, Scoriels et al. 2008). However, it is also important to keep in mind that meta-analyses tend to be biased, as studies with null findings are generally not published. It is likely that sample sizes will have to be increased in order to replicate previous findings and to generate accurate assessments of the effect sizes of different polymorphisms.

Go to:

Summary/Conclusions

The inability to consistently control behavior concurrent with increased sensation seeking persists in adolescence, leading to increases in risk taking behaviors. Although these behaviors may be mediated by non-biological factors, we must characterize the biological mechanisms driving development in order to better understand their consequences. Evidence points to a protracted development of brain systems including PFC and the striatum throughout childhood and adolescence. These systems support motivationally driven behaviors and may contribute to vulnerabilities in the emergence of psychopathology. The PFC and striatum support incentive driven behaviors through their unique interconnectivity, which is modulated by the function of DA. DA availability and signaling is heightened during the adolescent period and may promote novelty seeking in an adaptive fashion in order to gain skills that support adult survival. However, exaggerated DA levels in both striatum and PFC in adolescence may result in an increased sensitivity to rewards coupled with poor executive regulation of impulse driven behaviors, thereby increasing vulnerability for risk-taking behaviors. Despite general patterns of maturational change in DA, there is great variability in adolescent behaviors, which generates questions about the biological mechanisms that underlie this variability, a line of research yet to be explored. Gene expression is one of the primary sources of variability, acting through cellular and system-level neural processes to produce complex phenomena that manifest in behavioral function and dysfunction. The majority of imaging genetics research to date has focused on differences between genotypes in adulthood or within discrete age groups, despite growing evidence that brain systems continue to reorganize across the lifespan and that gene effects likely manifest differently at different stages. Identifying the nature of these changing trajectories will be more informative to the study of the brain than measuring static differences within age groups. The limited developmental imaging genetics research (i.e. (Dumontheil, Roggeman et al. 2011) has suggested that the direction of gene effects on brain function may change over development as brain systems reorganize. Future imaging genetics work should study gene effects across development (and the life span), ideally in a longitudinal fashion. This can have strong implications for understanding the neurobiology of heightened risk taking during adolescence, recognizing vulnerabilities for the emergence of psychopathology, developing age specific treatments, and the identifying the individual pathways that lead to behavioral outcomes in adulthood.

Highlights

  • Frontostriatal systems underlying motivated behaviors are immature in adolescence
  • The dopamine system undergoes significant reorganization over adolescence
  • Imaging genetics can be used to study the biological basis of variability in brain function
  • Imaging genetics may be valuable to study the influence of dopamine in adolescence

Go to:

Footnotes

Publisher’s Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Go to:

Reference List

  1. Aarts E, Roelofs A, et al. Striatal dopamine mediates the interface between motivational and cognitive control in humans: evidence from genetic imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(9):1943–1951. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  2. Adriani W, Chiarotti F, et al. Elevated novelty seeking and peculiar d-amphetamine sensitization in periadolescent mice compared with adult mice. Behav Neurosci. 1998;112(5):1152–1166. [PubMed]
  3. Adriani W, Laviola G. A unique hormonal and behavioral hyporesponsivity to both forced novelty and d-amphetamine in periadolescent mice. Neuropharmacology. 2000;39(2):334–346. [PubMed]
  4. Alexander GE, DeLong MR, et al. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1986;9:357–381. [PubMed]
  5. Andersen SL. Trajectories of brain development: point of vulnerability or window of opportunity? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27(1–2):3–18. [PubMed]
  6. Andersen SL, Thompson AP, et al. Pubertal changes in gonadal hormones do not underlie adolescent dopamine receptor overproduction. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27(6):683–691. [PubMed]
  7. Andersen SL, Thompson AT, et al. Dopamine receptor pruning in prefrontal cortex during the periadolescent period in rats. Synapse. 2000;37(2):167–169. [PubMed]
  8. Anderson SA, Classey JD, et al. Synchronous development of pyramidal neuron dendritic spines and parvalbumin-immunoreactive chandelier neuron axon terminals in layer III of monkey prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience. 1995;67(1):7–22. [PubMed]
  9. Apud JA, Mattay V, et al. Tolcapone improves cognition and cortical information processing in normal human subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32(5):1011–1020. [PubMed]
  10. Arinami T, Gao M, et al. A functional polymorphism in the promoter region of the dopamine D2 receptor gene is associated with schizophrenia. Human Molecular Genetics. 1997;6(4):577–582. [PubMed]
  11. Asghari V, Sanyal S, et al. Modulation of intracellular cyclic AMP levels by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1995;65(3):1157–1165. [PubMed]
  12. Badanich KA, Adler KJ, et al. Adolescents differ from adults in cocaine conditioned place preference and cocaine-induced dopamine in the nucleus accumbens septi. Eur J Pharmacol. 2006;550(1–3):95–106. [PubMed]
  13. Barnett JH, Scoriels L, et al. Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-Omethyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(2):137–144. [PubMed]
  14. Bedard AC, Schulz KP, et al. Dopamine transporter gene variation modulates activation of striatum in youth with ADHD. Neuroimage. 2010;53(3):935–942. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  15. Benes FM, Taylor JB, et al. Convergence and plasticity of monoaminergic systems in the medial prefrontal cortex during the postnatal period: implications for the development of psychopathology. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10(10):1014–1027. [PubMed]
  16. Benes FM, Turtle M, et al. Myelination of a key relay zone in the hippocampal formation occurs in the human brain during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51(6):477–484. [PubMed]
  17. Bertolino A, Blasi G, et al. Additive effects of genetic variation in dopamine regulating genes on working memory cortical activity in human brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(15):3918–3922. [PubMed]
  18. Bertolino A, Di GA, et al. Epistasis between dopamine regulating genes identifies a nonlinear response of the human hippocampus during memory tasks. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(3):226–234. [PubMed]
  19. Bigos KL, Hariri AR. Neuroimaging: technologies at the interface of genes, brain, and behavior. Neuroimaging Clin.N.Am. 2007;17(4):459–467. viii. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  20. Bilder RM, Volavka J, et al. Neurocognitive correlates of the COMT Val(158)Met polymorphism in chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52(7):701–707. [PubMed]
  21. Bjork JM, Knutson B, et al. Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: similarities and differences from young adults. J Neurosci. 2004;24(8):1793–1802. [PubMed]
  22. Bjork JM, Smith AR, et al. Adolescents, adults and rewards: comparing motivational neurocircuitry recruitment using fMRI. PLoS.One. 2010;5(7):e11440. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  23. Blakemore SJ, Robbins TW. Decision-making in the adolescent brain. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(9):1184–1191. [PubMed]
  24. Braet W, Johnson KA, et al. fMRI activation during response inhibition and error processing: the role of the DAT1 gene in typically developing adolescents and those diagnosed with ADHD. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(7):1641–1650. [PubMed]
  25. Brenhouse HC, Andersen SL. Delayed extinction and stronger reinstatement of cocaine conditioned place preference in adolescent rats, compared to adults. Behav Neurosci. 2008;122(2):460–465. [PubMed]
  26. Brenhouse HC, Sonntag KC, et al. Transient D1 dopamine receptor expression on prefrontal cortex projection neurons: relationship to enhanced motivational salience of drug cues in adolescence. J Neurosci. 2008;28(10):2375–2382. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  27. Brown SM, Hariri AR. Neuroimaging studies of serotonin gene polymorphisms: exploring the interplay of genes, brain, and behavior. Cogn Affect.Behav.Neurosci. 2006;6(1):44–52. [PubMed]
  28. Cagniard B, Balsam PD, et al. Mice with chronically elevated dopamine exhibit enhanced motivation, but not learning, for a food reward. Neuropsychopharmacology : of ficial publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(7):1362–1370. [PubMed]
  29. Cagniard B, Beeler JA, et al. Dopamine scales performance in the absence of new learning. Neuron. 2006;51(5):541–547. [PubMed]
  30. Camara E, Kramer UM, et al. The effects of COMT (Val108/158Met) and DRD4 (SNP-521) dopamine genotypes on brain activations related to valence and magnitude of rewards. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20(8):1985–1996. [PubMed]
  31. Cao J, Lotfipour S, et al. Adolescent maturation of cocaine-sensitive neural mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32(11):2279–2289. [PubMed]
  32. Casey BJ, Getz S, et al. The adolescent brain. Dev Rev. 2008;28(1):62–77. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  33. Casey BJ, Soliman F, et al. Imaging genetics and development: challenges and promises. Hum.Brain Mapp. 2010;31(6):838–851. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  34. Centonze D, Grande C, et al. Receptor subtypes involved in the presynaptic and postsynaptic actions of dopamine on striatal interneurons. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2003;23(15):6245–6254. [PubMed]
  35. Cepeda C, Levine MS. Dopamine and N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor interactions in the neostriatum. Dev Neurosci. 1998;20(1):1–18. [PubMed]
  36. Chambers RA, Taylor JR, et al. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(6):1041–1052. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  37. Chen J, Lipska BK, et al. Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-Omethyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2004;75(5):807–821. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  38. Cohen MX, Young J, et al. Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics predict neural reward responses. Brain research. Cognitive brain research. 2005;25(3):851–861. [PubMed]
  39. Congdon E, Constable RT, et al. Influence of SLC6A3 and COMT variation on neural activation during response inhibition. Biological Psychology. 2009;81(3):144–152. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  40. Congdon E, Lesch KP, et al. Analysis of DRD4 and DAT polymorphisms and behavioral inhibition in healthy adults: implications for impulsivity. American journal of medical genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric genetics : the official publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics. 2008;147B(1):27–32. [PubMed]
  41. Cools R. Role of dopamine in the motivational and cognitive control of behavior. Neuroscientist. 2008;14(4):381–395. [PubMed]
  42. Cools R, D’Esposito M. Inverted-u-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(12):e113–e125. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  43. Cools R, Frank MJ, et al. Striatal dopamine predicts outcome-specific reversal learning and its sensitivity to dopaminergic drug administration. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(5):1538–1543. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  44. Cormier F, Muellner J, et al. Genetics of impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm. 2013;120(4):665–671. [PubMed]
  45. Coulter CL, Happe HK, et al. Postnatal development of the dopamine transporter: a quantitative autoradiographic study. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1996;92(2):172–181. [PubMed]
  46. Dahl RE. Adolescent brain development: a period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Keynote address. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021:1–22. [PubMed]
  47. Depue RA, Collins PF. Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. The Behavioral and brain sciences. 1999;22(3):491–517. discussion 518–469. [PubMed]
  48. Di Martino A, Scheres A, et al. Functional connectivity of human striatum: a resting state FMRI study. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18(12):2735–2747. [PubMed]
  49. Diamond A, Briand L, et al. Genetic and neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cognitive functions in children. Am.J.Psychiatry. 2004;161(1):125–132. [PubMed]
  50. Doremus TL, Brunell SC, et al. Anxiogenic effects during withdrawal from acute ethanol in adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2003;75(2):411–418. [PubMed]
  51. Drabant EM, Hariri AR, et al. Catechol O-methyltransferase val158met genotype and neural mechanisms related to affective arousal and regulation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(12):1396–1406. [PubMed]
  52. Dreher JC, Kohn P, et al. Variation in dopamine genes influences responsivity of the human reward system. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(2):617–622. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  53. Dumontheil I, Roggeman C, et al. Influence of the COMT Genotype on Working Memory and Brain Activity Changes During Development. Biol Psychiatry. 2011 [PubMed]
  54. Durstewitz D, Seamans JK. The computational role of dopamine D1 receptors in working memory. Neural networks : the official journal of the International Neural Network Society. 2002;15(4–6):561–572. [PubMed]
  55. Durstewitz D, Seamans JK, et al. Dopamine-mediated stabilization of delay-period activity in a network model of prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;83(3):1733–1750. [PubMed]
  56. Durston S, Davidson MC, et al. A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Dev Sci. 2006;9(1):1–8. [PubMed]
  57. Durston S, Fossella JA, et al. Dopamine transporter genotype conveys familial risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder through striatal activation. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47(1):61–67. [PubMed]
  58. Egan MF, Goldberg TE, et al. Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A. 2001;98(12):6917–6922. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  59. Eley TC, Lichtenstein P, et al. A longitudinal behavioral genetic analysis of the etiology of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior. Dev.Psychopathol. 2003;15(2):383–402. [PubMed]
  60. Enoch MA, Schuckit MA, et al. Genetics of alcoholism using intermediate phenotypes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2003;27(2):169–176. [PubMed]
  61. Erickson SL, Lewis DA. Postnatal development of parvalbumin- and GABA transporter-immunoreactive axon terminals in monkey prefrontal cortex. J.Comp Neurol. 2002;448(2):186–202. [PubMed]
  62. Ernst M, Daniele T, et al. New perspectives on adolescent motivated behavior: attention and conditioning. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;1(4):377–389. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  63. Ernst M, Fudge JL. A developmental neurobiological model of motivated behavior: anatomy, connectivity and ontogeny of the triadic nodes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33(3):367–382. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  64. Ernst M, Nelson EE, et al. Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. Neuroimage. 2005;25(4):1279–1291. [PubMed]
  65. Ernst M, Pine DS, et al. Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychol Med. 2006;36(3):299–312. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  66. Fair DA, Cohen AL, et al. Functional brain networks develop from a”local to distributed organization. PLoS Comput Biol. 2009;5(5):e1000381. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  67. Falkner FT, Tanner JM. Human growth : a comprehensive treatise. New York: Plenum Press; 1986.
  68. Farde L, Halldin C, et al. PET analysis of human dopamine receptor subtypes using 11C-SCH 23390 and 11C-raclopride. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1987;92(3):278–284. [PubMed]
  69. Fisher PM, Munoz KE, et al. Identification of neurogenetic pathways of risk for psychopathology. Am.J.Med.Genet.C.Semin.Med.Genet. 2008;148(2):147–153. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  70. Flint J, Munafo MR. The endophenotype concept in psychiatric genetics. Psychol Med. 2007;37(2):163–180. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  71. Floresco SB, West AR, et al. Afferent modulation of dopamine neuron firing differentially regulates tonic and phasic dopamine transmission. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6(9):968–973. [PubMed]
  72. Forbes EE, Brown SM, et al. Genetic variation in components of dopamine neurotransmission impacts ventral striatal reactivity associated with impulsivity. Mol.Psychiatry. 2009;14(1):60–70. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  73. Fouriezos G, Hansson P, et al. Neuroleptic-induced attenuation of brain stimulation reward in rats. J.Comp Physiol Psychol. 1978;92(4):661–671. [PubMed]
  74. Frank MJ, Hutchison K. Genetic contributions to avoidance-based decisions: striatal D2 receptor polymorphisms. Neuroscience. 2009;164(1):131–140. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  75. Frantz KJ, O’Dell LE, et al. Behavioral and neurochemical responses to cocaine in periadolescent and adult rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32(3):625–637. [PubMed]
  76. Fuke S, Suo S, et al. The VNTR polymorphism of the human dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene affects gene expression. Pharmacogenomics. 2001;1(2):152–156. [PubMed]
  77. Galineau L, Kodas E, et al. Ontogeny of the dopamine and serotonin transporters in the rat brain: an autoradiographic study. Neurosci Lett. 2004;363(3):266–271. [PubMed]
  78. Galvan A, Hare TA, et al. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J Neurosci. 2006;26(25):6885–6892. [PubMed]
  79. Garcia-Garcia M, Barcelo F, et al. The role of the dopamine transporter DAT1 genotype on the neural correlates of cognitive flexibility. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;31(4):754–760. [PubMed]
  80. Geier CF, Luna B. The maturation of incentive processing and cognitive control. Pharmacol.Biochem.Behav. 2009;93(3):212–221. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  81. Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(10):861–863. [PubMed]
  82. Gilsbach S, Neufang S, et al. Effects of the DRD4 genotype on neural networks associated with executive functions in children and adolescents. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;2(4):417–427. [PubMed]
  83. Goldberg TE, Egan MF, et al. Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia. Arch.Gen.Psychiatry. 2003;60(9):889–896. [PubMed]
  84. Goldman-Rakic PS. Parallel systems in the cerebral cortex: the topography of cognition. In: Arbib MA, Robinson JA, editors. Natural and artificial parallel computation. New York: MIT Press; 1990. pp. 155–176.
  85. Goldman-Rakic PS, Brown RM. Postnatal development of monoamine content and synthesis in the cerebral cortex of rhesus monkeys. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1982;256(3):339–349. [PubMed]
  86. Goto Y, Grace AA. Dopamine-dependent interactions between limbic and prefrontal cortical plasticity in the nucleus accumbens: disruption by cocaine sensitization. Neuron. 2005;47(2):255–266. [PubMed]
  87. Grace AA, Floresco SB, et al. Regulation of firing of dopaminergic neurons and control of goal-directed behaviors. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(5):220–227. [PubMed]
  88. Hariri AR, Lewis DA. Genetics and the future of clinical psychiatry. Am.J.Psychiatry. 2006;163(10):1676–1678. [PubMed]
  89. Hariri AR, Weinberger DR. Imaging genomics. Br.Med.Bull. 2003;65:259–270. [PubMed]
  90. Haycock JW, Becker L, et al. Marked disparity between age-related changes in dopamine and other presynaptic dopaminergic markers in human striatum. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2003;87(3):574–585. [PubMed]
  91. Hedner T, Iversen K, et al. Central GABA mechanisms during postnatal development in the rat: neurochemical characteristics. J Neural Transm. 1984;59(2):105–118. [PubMed]
  92. Hong J, Shu-Leong H, et al. Distribution of catechol-O-methyltransferase expression in human central nervous system. Neuroreport. 1998;9(12):2861–2864. [PubMed]
  93. Huttenlocher PR. Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28(6):517–527. [PubMed]
  94. Hwang K, Velanova K, et al. Strengthening of top-down frontal cognitive control networks underlying the development of inhibitory control: a functional magnetic resonance imaging effective connectivity study. J Neurosci. 2010;30(46):15535–15545. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  95. Jaber M, Bloch B, et al. Behavioral, cellular and molecular consequences of the dopamine transporter gene inactivation. C R Seances Soc Biol Fil. 1998;192(6):1127–1137. [PubMed]
  96. Jernigan TL, Trauner DA, et al. Maturation of human cerebrum observed in vivo during adolescence. Brain. 1991;114(P Á 5):2037–2049. [PubMed]
  97. Karayiorgou M, Altemus M, et al. Genotype determining low catechol-Omethyltransferase activity as a risk factor for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A. 1997;94(9):4572–4575. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  98. Kendler KS, Neale MC. Endophenotype: a conceptual analysis. Molecular Psychiatry. 2010;15(8):789–797. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  99. Kereszturi E, Kiraly O, et al. No direct effect of the-521 C/T polymorphism in the human dopamine D4 receptor gene promoter on transcriptional activity. BMC molecular biology. 2006;7:18. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  100. Kirsch P, Reuter M, et al. Imaging gene-substance interactions: the effect of the DRD2 TaqIA polymorphism and the dopamine agonist bromocriptine on the brain activation during the anticipation of reward. Neurosci Lett. 2006;405(3):196–201. [PubMed]
  101. Lambe EK, Krimer LS, et al. Differential postnatal development of catecholamine and serotonin inputs to identified neurons in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20(23):8780–8787. [PubMed]
  102. Laviola G, Adriani W, et al. Psychobiological risk factors for vulnerability to psychostimulants in human adolescents and animal models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;23(7):993–1010. [PubMed]
  103. Laviola G, Macri S, et al. Risk-taking behavior in adolescent mice: psychobiological determinants and early epigenetic influence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27(1–2):19–31. [PubMed]
  104. Le Moal M, Simon H. Mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic network: functional and regulatory roles. Physiological Reviews. 1991;71(1):155–234. [PubMed]
  105. Lee SS, Lahey BB, et al. Association of dopamine transporter genotype with disruptive behavior disorders in an eight-year longitudinal study of children and adolescents. Am.J.Med.Genet.B Neuropsychiatr.Genet. 2007;144B(3):310–317. [PubMed]
  106. Levin ED, Rezvani AH, et al. Adolescent-onset nicotine self-administration modeled in female rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;169(2):141–149. [PubMed]
  107. Lewis DA. Development of the prefrontal cortex during adolescence: insights into vulnerable neural circuits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;16(6):385–398. [PubMed]
  108. Lewis DA, Gonzalez-Burgos G. Intrinsic excitatory connections in the prefrontal cortex and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Brain Res Bull. 2000;52(5):309–317. [PubMed]
  109. Lidow MS, Goldman-Rakic PS, et al. Synchronized overproduction of neurotransmitter receptors in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1991;88(22):10218–10221. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  110. Lidow MS, Rakic P. Scheduling of monoaminergic neurotransmitter receptor expression in the primate neocortex during postnatal development. Cereb Cortex. 1992;2(5):401–416. [PubMed]
  111. Liston C, Watts R, et al. Frontostriatal microstructure modulates efficient recruitment of cognitive control. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16(4):553–560. [PubMed]
  112. Logothetis NK, Pauls M JA, et al. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature. 2001;412:150–157. [PubMed]
  113. Luciana M, Wahlstrom D, et al. Dopaminergic modulation of incentive motivation in adolescence: age-related changes in signaling, individual differences, and implications for the development of self-regulation. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(3):844–861. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  114. Malhotra AK, Kestler LJ, et al. A functional polymorphism in the COMT gene and performance on a test of prefrontal cognition. Am.J.Psychiatry. 2002;159(4):652–654. [PubMed]
  115. Mathews IZ, McCormick CM. Female and male rats in late adolescence differ from adults in amphetamine-induced locomotor activity, but not in conditioned place preference for amphetamine. Behav Pharmacol. 2007;18(7):641–650. [PubMed]
  116. Matsumoto M, Weickert CS, et al. Catechol O-methyltransferase mRNA expression in human and rat brain: evidence for a role in cortical neuronal function. Neuroscience. 2003;116(1):127–137. [PubMed]
  117. Mattay VS, Goldberg TE, et al. Catechol O-methyltransferase val158-met genotype and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A. 2003;100(10):6186–6191. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  118. McCutcheon JE, White FJ, et al. Individual differences in dopamine cell neuroadaptations following cocaine self-administration. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(8):801–803. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  119. Mechelli A, Tognin S, et al. Genetic vulnerability to affective psychopathology in childhood: a combined voxel-based morphometry and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(3):231–237. [PubMed]
  120. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Kohn PD, et al. Midbrain dopamine and prefrontal function in humans: interaction and modulation by COMT genotype. Nat.Neurosci. 2005;8(5):594–596. [PubMed]
  121. Mill J, Asherson P, et al. Expression of the dopamine transporter gene is regulated by the 3′ UTR VNTR: Evidence from brain and lymphocytes using quantitative RT-PCR. Am J Med Genet. 2002;114(8):975–979. [PubMed]
  122. Moll GH, Mehnert C, et al. Age-associated changes in the densities of presynaptic monoamine transporters in different regions of the rat brain from early juvenile life to late adulthood. Brain research. Developmental brain research. 2000;119(2):251–257. [PubMed]
  123. Montague DM, Lawler CP, et al. Developmental regulation of the dopamine D1 receptor in human caudate and putamen. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21(5):641–649. [PubMed]
  124. Munafo MR, Bowes L, et al. Lack of association of the COMT (Val158/108 Met) gene and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of case-control studies. Mol.Psychiatry. 2005;10(8):765–770. [PubMed]
  125. Munafo MR, Brown SM, et al. Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(9):852–857. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  126. Nelson EE, Leibenluft E, et al. The social re-orientation of adolescence: a neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychol Med. 2005;35(2):163–174. [PubMed]
  127. Nemoda Z, Szekely A, et al. Psychopathological aspects of dopaminergic gene polymorphisms in adolescence and young adulthood. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(8):1665–1686. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  128. Nikolova YS, Ferrell RE, et al. Multilocus genetic profile for dopamine signaling predicts ventral striatum reactivity. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(9):1940–1947. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  129. Niv Y, Daw ND, et al. Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;191(3):507–520. [PubMed]
  130. Noble EP. The DRD2 gene in psychiatric and neurological disorders and its phenotypes. Pharmacogenomics. 2000;1(3):309–333. [PubMed]
  131. O’Donnell P. Adolescent maturation of cortical dopamine. Neurotoxicity research. 2010;18(3–4):306–312. [PubMed]
  132. Okuyama Y, Ishiguro H, et al. A genetic polymorphism in the promoter region of DRD4 associated with expression and schizophrenia. Biochemical and biophysical research communications. 1999;258(2):292–295. [PubMed]
  133. Padmanabhan A. Developmental changes in brain function underlying the influence of reward processing on inhibitory control. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  134. Paloyelis Y, Mehta MA, et al. Striatal sensitivity during reward processing in attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(7):722–732. e729. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  135. Paus T. Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(2):60–68. [PubMed]
  136. Paus T, Keshavan M, et al. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(12):947–957. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  137. Pecina M, Mickey BJ, et al. DRD2 polymorphisms modulate reward and emotion processing, dopamine neurotransmission and openness to experience. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior. 2012 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  138. Perez-Edgar K, Hardee JE, et al. DRD4 and striatal modulation of the link between childhood behavioral inhibition and adolescent anxiety. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2013 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  139. Pfefferbaum A, Mathalon DH, et al. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Archives of Neurology. 1994;51(9):874–887. [PubMed]
  140. Philpot RM, Wecker L, et al. Repeated ethanol exposure during adolescence alters the developmental trajectory of dopaminergic output from the nucleus accumbens septi. International journal of developmental neuroscience : the official journal of the International Society for Developmental Neuroscience. 2009;27(8):805–815. [PubMed]
  141. Pijnenburg AJ, Honig WM, et al. Further investigations on the effects of ergometrine and other ergot derivatives following injection into the nucleus accumbens of the rat. Archives internationales de pharmacodynamie et de therapie. 1976;222(1):103–115. [PubMed]
  142. Pine DS. Brain development and the onset of mood disorders. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry. 2002;7(4):223–233. [PubMed]
  143. Plomin R, Haworth CM, et al. Common disorders are quantitative traits. Nature reviews. Genetics. 2009;10(12):872–878. [PubMed]
  144. Pohjalainen T, Nagren K, et al. The dopamine D2 receptor 5′-flanking variant,-141C Ins/Del, is not associated with reduced dopamine D2 receptor density in vivo. Pharmacogenetics. 1999;9(4):505–509. [PubMed]
  145. Postuma RB, Dagher A. Basal ganglia functional connectivity based on a metaanalysis of 126 positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging publications. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16(10):1508–1521. [PubMed]
  146. Rakic P, Bourgeois JP, et al. Concurrent overproduction of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. Science. 1986;232(4747):232–235. [PubMed]
  147. Raznahan A, Greenstein D, et al. Catechol-o-methyl transferase (COMT) val158met polymorphism and adolescent cortical development in patients with childhood-onset schizophrenia, their non-psychotic siblings, and healthy controls. Neuroimage. 2011;57(4):1517–1523. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  148. Robbins TW, Arnsten AF. The neuropsychopharmacology of fronto-executive function: monoaminergic modulation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:267–287. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  149. Romeo RD. Puberty: a period of both organizational and activational effects of steroid hormones on neurobehavioural development. J.Neuroendocrinol. 2003;15(12):1185–1192. [PubMed]
  150. Rosenberg DR, Lewis DA. Changes in the dopaminergic innervation of monkey prefrontal cortex during late postnatal development: a tyrosine hydroxylase immunohistochemical study. Biol Psychiatry. 1994;36(4):272–277. [PubMed]
  151. Rosenberg DR, Lewis DA. Postnatal maturation of the dopaminergic innervation of monkey prefrontal and motor cortices: a tyrosine hydroxylase immunohistochemical analysis. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1995;358(3):383–400. [PubMed]
  152. Schoots O, Van Tol HH. The human dopamine D4 receptor repeat sequences modulate expression. The pharmacogenomics journal. 2003;3(6):343–348. [PubMed]
  153. Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80(1):1–27. [PubMed]
  154. Schultz W. Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron. 2002;36(2):241–263. [PubMed]
  155. Seamans JK, Durstewitz D, et al. Dopamine D1/D5 receptor modulation of excitatory synaptic inputs to layer V prefrontal cortex neurons. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A. 2001;98(1):301–306. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  156. Seamans JK, Yang CR. The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Prog Neurobiol. 2004;74(1):1–58. [PubMed]
  157. Seeger G, Schloss P, et al. Marker gene polymorphisms in hyperkinetic disorder– predictors of clinical response to treatment with methylphenidate? Neurosci Lett. 2001;313(1–2):45–48. [PubMed]
  158. Seeman P, Bzowej NH, et al. Human brain dopamine receptors in children and aging adults. Synapse. 1987;1(5):399–404. [PubMed]
  159. Shaw P, Gornick M, et al. Polymorphisms of the dopamine D4 receptor, clinical outcome, and cortical structure in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch.Gen.Psychiatry. 2007;64(8):921–931. [PubMed]
  160. Shram MJ, Funk D, et al. Periadolescent and adult rats respond differently in tests measuring the rewarding and aversive effects of nicotine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;186(2):201–208. [PubMed]
  161. Sisk CL, Zehr JL. Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2005;26(3–4):163–174. [PubMed]
  162. Spear LP. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24(4):417–463. [PubMed]
  163. Spear LP. Alcohol’s effects on adolescents. Alcohol research & health : the journal of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. 2002;26(4):287–291. [PubMed]
  164. Spear LP. Rewards, aversions and affect in adolescence: emerging convergences across laboratory animal and human data. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;1(4):392–400. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  165. Spear LP, Brake SC. Periadolescence: age-dependent behavior and psychopharmacological responsivity in rats. Dev Psychobiol. 1983;16(2):83–109. [PubMed]
  166. Spear LP, Shalaby IA, et al. Chronic administration of haloperidol during development: behavioral and psychopharmacological effects. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1980;70(1):47–58. [PubMed]
  167. Steinberg L. Risk taking in adolescence: what changes, and why? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021:51–58. [PubMed]
  168. Steinberg L, Cauffman E, et al. Are adolescents less mature than adults?: minors’ access to abortion, the juvenile death penalty, and the alleged APA “flip-flop”. Am Psychol. 2009;64(7):583–594. [PubMed]
  169. Stelzel C, Basten U, et al. Frontostriatal involvement in task switching depends on genetic differences in d2 receptor density. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2010;30(42):14205–14212. [PubMed]
  170. Stevens MC, Pearlson GD, et al. Changes in the interaction of resting-state neural networks from adolescence to adulthood. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30(8):2356–2366. [PubMed]
  171. Stice E, Dagher A. Genetic variation in dopaminergic reward in humans. Forum Nutr. 2010;63:176–185. [PubMed]
  172. Stice E, Spoor S, et al. Relation between obesity and blunted striatal response to food is moderated by TaqIA A1 allele. Science. 2008;322(5900):449–452. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  173. Stice E, Yokum S, et al. Reward circuitry responsivity to food predicts future increases in body mass: moderating effects of DRD2 and DRD4. Neuroimage. 2010;50(4):1618–1625. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  174. Stice E, Yokum S, et al. Multilocus genetic composite reflecting dopamine signaling capacity predicts reward circuitry responsivity. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2012;32(29):10093–10100. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  175. Sturman DA, Moghaddam B. The neurobiology of adolescence: changes in brain architecture, functional dynamics, and behavioral tendencies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(8):1704–1712. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  176. Tarazi FI, Tomasini EC, et al. Postnatal development of dopamine and serotonin transporters in rat caudate-putamen and nucleus accumbens septi. Neurosci Lett. 1998;254(1):21–24. [PubMed]
  177. Teicher MH, Andersen SL, et al. Evidence for dopamine receptor pruning between adolescence and adulthood in striatum but not nucleus accumbens. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1995;89(2):167–172. [PubMed]
  178. Teicher MH, Barber NI, et al. Developmental differences in acute nigrostriatal and mesocorticolimbic system response to haloperidol. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1993;9(2):147–156. [PubMed]
  179. Tekin S, Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical neuronal circuits and clinical neuropsychiatry: an update. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2002;53(2):647–654. [PubMed]
  180. Thirion B, Pinel P, et al. Analysis of a large fMRI cohort: Statistical and methodological issues for group analyses. Neuroimage. 2007;35(1):105–120. [PubMed]
  181. Thomason ME, Dougherty RF, et al. COMT genotype affects prefrontal white matter pathways in children and adolescents. Neuroimage. 2010;53(3):926–934. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  182. Thomason ME, Waugh CE, et al. COMT genotype and resting brain perfusion in children. Neuroimage. 2009;48(1):217–222. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  183. Tseng KY, O’Donnell P. Dopamine modulation of prefrontal cortical interneurons changes during adolescence. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(5):1235–1240. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  184. Tunbridge EM. The catechol-o-methyltransferase gene its regulation and polymorphisms. Int.Rev.Neurobiol. 2010;95:7–27. [PubMed]
  185. Tura E, Turner JA, et al. Multivariate analyses suggest genetic impacts on neurocircuitry in schizophrenia. Neuroreport. 2008;19(6):603–607. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  186. van de Giessen EM, de Win MM, et al. Striatal dopamine transporter availability associated with polymorphisms in the dopamine transporter gene SLC6A3. J.Nucl.Med. 2009;50(1):45–52. [PubMed]
  187. van der Schaaf ME, van Schouwenburg MR, et al. Establishing the Dopamine Dependency of Human Striatal Signals During Reward and Punishment Reversal Learning. Cerebral Cortex. 2012 [PubMed]
  188. van Leijenhorst L, Moor BG, et al. Adolescent risky decision-making: Neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. Neuroimage. 2010 [PubMed]
  189. Vandenbergh DJ, Persico AM, et al. Human dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) maps to chromosome 5p15.3 and displays a VNTR. Genomics. 1992;14(4):1104–1106. [PubMed]
  190. VanNess SH, Owens MJ, et al. The variable number of tandem repeats element in DAT1 regulates in vitro dopamine transporter density. BMC Genet. 2005;6:55. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  191. Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Sensitization to social anxiolytic effects of ethanol in adolescent and adult Sprague-Dawley rats after repeated ethanol exposure. Alcohol. 2010;44(1):99–110. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  192. Wahlstrom D, Collins P, et al. Developmental changes in dopamine neurotransmission in adolescence: behavioral implications and issues in assessment. Brain Cogn. 2010;72(1):146–159. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  193. Wahlstrom D, White T, et al. Neurobehavioral evidence for changes in dopamine system activity during adolescence. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34(5):631–648. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  194. Walters JT, Owen MJ. Endophenotypes in psychiatric genetics. Molecular Psychiatry. 2007;12(10):886–890. [PubMed]
  195. Weickert CS, Webster MJ, et al. Postnatal alterations in dopaminergic markers in the human prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience. 2007;144(3):1109–1119. [PubMed]
  196. Williams LM, Gatt JM, et al. The integrate model of emotion, thinking and self regulation: an application to the”paradox of aging. Journal of integrative neuroscience. 2008;7(3):367–404. [PubMed]
  197. Wise RA. Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(6):483–494. [PubMed]
  198. Yacubian J, Sommer T, et al. Gene-gene interaction associated with neural reward sensitivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(19):8125–8130. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  199. Yakovlev PI, Lecours AR. Regional Development of the Brain in Early Life. A. Minkowski. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific; 1967. The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain; pp. 3–70.
  200. Yurgelun-Todd D. Emotional and cognitive changes during adolescence. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17(2):251–257. [PubMed]
  201. Zald DH, Cowan RL, et al. Midbrain dopamine receptor availability is inversely associated with novelty-seeking traits in humans. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2008;28(53):14372–14378. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  202. Zhang Y, Bertolino A, et al. Polymorphisms in human dopamine D2 receptor gene affect gene expression, splicing, and neuronal activity during working memory. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A. 2007;104(51):20552–20557. [PMC free article] [PubMed]