Med Hypotheses. 2016 Aug;93:62-70. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2016.05.015.
Patrono E1, Gasbarri A2, Tomaz C3, Nishijo H4.
Article Outline
- Introduction
- A “temporal continuum” hypotheses involving the aberrant motivation, the hedonic dysregulation, and the aberrant learning
- Neuro-bio-physiological background of drug addiction “temporal continuum” hypotheses
- The neural basis of a drug motivated behavior
- The neural basis of a habit-learning drug behavior
- The legitimacy of the term “food addiction”
- The neural basis of food addiction
- Electrophysiological basis of food-directed behavior
- A new parallel addictive behavior
- Conclusions
- Authors and contributors
- Conflicts of interest
- References
Abstract
Keywords:
Drug/food addiction, Motivation, Habit learning, Hedonic dysregulation, Transitionality, Reward system
Introduction
Addiction, from the Latin “addictus” (“slave to debt” or “subjugate”), is a chronic compulsion and relapsing disorder that affects people more psychologically than physically. It is a chronic condition involving several brain areas and circuits, which encode several functions such as reward, motivation and memory. An addict gradually focuses most of his energy on the searching for, finding, and subsequently obtaining and using substances of abuse. This happens even in spite of illness, failures in life and disrupted relationships.
Recently, addiction was defined in DSM-V as a “pathological pattern of use of a substance” characterized by the loss of control on drug-taking-related behaviors, the pursuance of those behaviors even in the presence of negative consequences, and a strong motivated activity to assume substances [1]. The loss of control, the pursuance, and strong motivated activity to assume substances can be analyzed and conceptualized from psychological to biological-molecular level.
Three different theories guide experimental research on drug addiction [[2], [3], [4]]. Each of these theories consider singles features, such as an aberrant motivation [2], a hedonic dysregulation [3], and an aberrant habit learning [4] as the main actor to explain the entire process of the addictive behaviors. The major goal of this study is to present a new hypotheses of transitionality from a controlled use to abuse of addictive substances trough the overview of the three different theories, considering all the single features of each single theory together on the same “temporal continuum” from use to abuse of addictive substances.
Here we overview three major psychological hypotheses that try to explain the passage from casual use to abuse of pharmacological substances: the incentive-sensitization theory, the hedonic dysregulation theory, and the habit-based learning theory
The “incentive-sensitization” theory
In psychology, motivation is generally considered the internal condition that guides and modulates the behavior of an individual, toward a goal. The psychological processes guiding addiction behavior can be studied through motivational notions, understanding which brain systems are involved. Compulsive drug seeking/taking behavior and relapse (throughout exposure to stimuli associated with the substance or due to stress) are attributable to a change in the motivational system and the appetitive phase (wanting). Berridge and Robinson explained this phenomenon with the “incentive-sensitization theory” [2]. They suggest that chronic use of a drug leads to increasing neurological change within the reward system, sensitizing the system to drugs and associated stimuli. The enhancement of drug-stimuli pairings increases the incentive value of the stimuli, producing a “transitionality” in drug users that want drugs, even though they don’t get the like from them [5] (Fig. 1). Fig. 1 shows how liking and wanting can follow different psychological/brain pathways through the difference in memory comparison. Although this theory explains many aspects of human addiction, such as the excessive search for a drug, intense craving, and relapse, it cannot solely explain the main feature of drug addiction: the inability of addicts to regulate or stop the use of a drug, despite negative consequences and the self-destructive nature of its prolonged use. Drug addiction is a complex psychopathology characterized, at least partly, by drug-induced pleasure, drug-associated memories, and drug-related emotional traits that are connected to the “liking” stimuli [[6], [7]]. An imbalance of both “wanting” (e.g. incentive-sensitization) and “liking” can have a role in the induction of addictive behaviors [8]. However, even though this theory does not reject drug-induced pleasure, withdrawal, or habits as reasons for the drug seeking/taking behavior, it hypothesizes that other factors, such as a sensitized wanting, could better explain compulsion and relapse in addiction.
Fig. 1
Incentive salience model of incentive motivation. “Liking” and “wanting” correspond to separate psychological and neurological systems. Conditioned stimuli (CS) and unconditioned stimuli (US) produce a memory comparison. DA projections to the NAc and neostriatum generate wanting (incentive-salience aspects of motivation). Conversely, DA does not project directly to the NAc and neostriatum relatively to liking (hedonia) and to associative-learning of rewards. Further cognitive elaborations are required for personal evaluation of pleasure and motivation, in order to have consciousness of emotions underlying “liking” and “wanting”.
View Large Image | View Hi-Res Image | Download PowerPoint Slide
The “hedonic dysregulation” theory
The role of “sensitization” in addiction has been explained as a smooth move to an “incentive-salience” state. Initial use is promoted by the hedonically rewarding properties of the drug, such as a euphoric high, while addictive use is hypothesized to grow by “negative reinforcement” [10]. Negative reinforcement is a process by which discharge of aversive stimuli, such as a negative emotional state of withdrawal, increases the number of drug intake [3]. In order to avoid dysphoria and discomfort, drug users take pharmacological substances [11]. However, drug users proceed from casual use to addiction, and the factors promoting “transitionality” in drug use are hypothesized to shift from impulsivity in the early periods, to compulsivity in last periods. Craving (an intense and powerful desire) has a crucial role in addiction, and is considered a part of the three components: “preoccupation/anticipation”, “binge/intoxication”, and “withdrawal/negative effect” [10]. The three stages are interactive with each other, deepening in intense, dysregulating the hedonic homeostasis of reward system, and finally bringing the user to addiction [[3], [10]] (Fig. 2). Fig. 2 describes the top–down addiction cycle in which the “preoccupation/anticipation” stage as an overwhelming urge to use drugs even if his or her life are plenty of responsibilities and human relationships. The “binge/intoxication” stage specifies the necessity of big amounts of drugs in order to experience the same level of hedonic effects. “Withdrawal/negative effect” refers to the psycho-physical effects induced by the absence of a continuous drug use, that need medical care (e.g. pharmacological use of methadone).
Fig. 2
Spiralling in a top–down vicious circle. Diagram describes the top–down addiction cycle. Craving is crucially involved in the process where a occasional drug use can transitionally lead to abuse, and subsequently to the relapse. This is explained through three factors: “preoccupation/anticipation”, “binge/intoxication”, and “withdrawal/negative” effect. These three stages interact with each other, becoming more intense, dysregulating the hedonic homeostasis of the reward system, and leading to the pathological state known as addiction.
View Large Image | View Hi-Res Image | Download PowerPoint Slide
The hedonic dysregulation theory elucidates the passage from use to abuse of drugs such as a “top–down vicious circle”, considering the key role of a sort of imbalance in hedonic status of drug users [3]. However, the theory cannot solely explain the role of other main features of drug addiction such as an abnormal sensitization to the substance and the instrumental behaviors to obtain the substance. The mesolimbic reward circuit was originally believed to encode simply the hedonic impact related to drug experiences. Recently, it is considered that this circuit is functionally more complex, encoding attention, expectancy of reward, and incentive motivation [12].
The “habit-based learning” theory
Everitt considers drug addiction the final stage of several transitional steps from the initial and controlled use of a substance [[13], [18], [19]] (Fig. 3). Fig. 3 describes the following steps through the drug addiction. When the substance is taken voluntarily for its incentive effect, seeking-behavior progressively becomes a “habit”, through a gradual loss of control. Thus, the stimulus–response mechanism plays a crucial role in the maintenance of an instrumental behavior. Finally, the capacity of the stimulus (substance) to act as reinforcement (conditioned reinforcer) exerts a kind of control over the seeking/taking behavior. Thus, drug addiction may start as a “goal-directed behavior”; later, with the maintenance of the “instrumental behavior”, it could turn into a “habitual behavior”, inducing a form of learning based on the habit (habit-based learning) [[13], [16], [18]].
Fig. 3
Following steps from use to abuse of substances. According to Everitt and colleagues, drug addiction is a series of steps that are followed by an initial, voluntary and emotionally activating use of addictive substances up to a loss of control on the consumption of the same substances through a change of the role of conditioned reinforcer. Specifically, when the substance is taken voluntarily for its incentive effect, seeking-behavior progressively becomes a “habit”, through a gradual loss of control. Thus, the stimulus–response mechanism plays a crucial role in the maintenance of an instrumental behavior. Finally, the capacity of the stimulus (substance) to act as reinforcement (conditioned reinforcer) exerts a kind of control over the seeking/taking behavior.
View Large Image | View Hi-Res Image | Download PowerPoint Slide
A “temporal continuum” hypotheses involving the aberrant motivation, the hedonic dysregulation, and the aberrant learning
This study aims to evaluate the three major theories of drug addiction from a new perspective of unity, through the theoretical hypotheses of a unique “temporal continuum” in which an “aberrant motivation”, a “hedonic dysregulation”, and an “aberrant learning” lie together in order to explain the transition from an occasional use to abuse of drugs (Fig. 4). Fig. 4 shows a hypothetical time-line in which the three major features are defined as a single “temporal continuum” from the first meet with the drugs to the addiction itself. A large body of literature assessed very well the role of each one of the three theories in drug addiction. Moreover, it has been defined that a progressive shift occurs from habit-driven to motivated-driven drug-seeking/taking behavior in which a hedonic dysregulation is firstly induced during habit learning and continues with the aberrant motivation to use drugs. The Pavlovian-instrumental transfer (PIT) design takes into account two conditions: (1) the Pavlovian processes that define sensitivity to the eventuality between a stimulus (S) and the reinforcers (R); and (2) the instrumental behaviors sensitive to the eventuality between active responses (R), and outcomes (O) [[20], [21]]. Neuro-bio-physiologically, this corresponds to a progressive shift from ventral to dorsal striatal control over drug-seeking/taking behavior [12]. Hence, it is possible to consider a unique “temporal continuum” in which (1) a progressively aberrant “habit-learning” occurs during casual drug use, wherein a “hedonic dysregulation” is activated and (2) lead to a progressively aberrant “salience-incentivation” inducing the drug-taking behavior. However, to our knowledge, there are no evidences of a unitary vision of the three theories through the “temporal continuum” hypotheses. Several human and animal studies have demonstrated that the time of reward has a strong role in reward processing [[22], [23]]. Furthermore, time windows and “reward rates” are of crucial importance for conditioning, and DA neurons are crucially involved in the processing of temporal information about the rewards. At a clinical level, this would also help to understand how and when to intervene along the temporal continuum from occasional use to abuse of pharmacological substances, and to produce new therapeutic strategies in order to avoid the insurgency of the pathological drug-seeking/taking behavior. Finally, motivation, hedonic dysregulation, and habit-based learning can be considered singular parts of a unique and complex drug-seeking/taking behavior.
Fig. 4
Hypothetical timeline of the “temporal continuum” hypotheses. Diagram describing a hypothetical time-line in which the three major features are defined as a single “temporal continuum” from the first meet with the drugs to the addiction. During this time, neurobehavioral changes act on the hedonic dysregulation and on the representation of the value of the drug inducing a habit-learning, and drastically losing the control over the drug intake.
View Large Image | View Hi-Res Image | Download PowerPoint Slide
Neuro-bio-physiological background of drug addiction “temporal continuum” hypotheses
Besides the behavioral criteria described above, several studies have drawn a connection between neural circuits activated in the drug-seeking/taking behavior. It is important to note that drug abuse activates several “cortico-subcortical” brain areas and neurotransmission circuits that are involved in the “drug-reinforcement”. In order to confirm the hypotheses that the three features enhanced in each single theory can lie in a single “temporal continuum” describing all together the passage from use to abuse of substances, neural basis of a drug-motivated behavior and a drug-habit-learned behavior will be revise
The neural basis of a drug motivated behavior
In contrast, the NAc “core” seems to be a crucial site that mediates the learned behaviors expression responding to stimuli that predict motivationally relevant events [[30], [37], [38], [39]]. Moreover, the expression of adaptive behaviors are likely modulated by DA release in NAc core during responses to stimuli predicting a rewarding event [[40], [41]]. In sum, DA might have two functions and might be crucial in “transitionality” from occasional drug use to abuse. The first alarm the organism to the apparition of new salient stimuli, and after induces learning neuroplasticity. The second is to alert the organism to the imminent apparition of an accustomed relevant event, and motivated on the basis of learned associations previously made through environmental stimuli-event prediction [42]. Finally, a series of parallel cortico–striato–pallido–cortical loops have been defined where the ventral striatum (VS), including NAc core is related to emotional learning; and the dorsal striatum (DS), including NAc shell is related to cognitive and motor functions [[43], [44]].
The neural basis of a habit-learning drug behavior
This raises the question of whether these selective neurochemical transmissions in the BLA and NAc core are parts of a brain subsystem within “limbic cortical-ventral striato-pallidal” circuitry [57]. In part, because the technique of the so-called “disconnection”, DS and VS interact with each other serially, in a wide range of functional settings, such as PIT on goal-directed behavior [21]. For long time, the VS has been suggested to keep in connection emotion, motivation, and action thanks to its major connections between structures such as the BLA and the orbitofrontal cortex (oFC) [[21], [57], [58]]. The NAc core is important in Pavlovian conditioning, as well as during interactions in “Pavlovian-instrumental” learning mechanisms related to involuntary behavior [[21], [38], [45]]. Conversely, it has been defined that DS has a role in cognitive and motor functions, giving a neurobiological base for both goal-directed and habitual control of “instrumental learning” [[59], [60], [61], [62]]. Pavlovian-instrumental learning sequential steps could be crucially important in the transitionality from occasional drug use to abuse, that could also involve compulsive drug-seeking/taking behavior [13].
Recently, several experimental and functional observations support the idea of common neural circuits forming a distinct entity into the basal forebrain, termed the “extended amygdala”. This circuit may be delegated to act on the motivational, emotional, and habitual effects of drug addiction [[63], [64], [65], [66]]. The extended amygdala is comprised of several basal forebrain structures such as the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), the central medial amygdala (CeA), and the NAc shell [[63], [64]]. These structures have similarities in morphology, immunohistochemistry, and connectivity [[65], [66]], and they receive afferent connections from limbic structures such as the hippocampus (HP), and BLA. Extended amygdala has key parts that include neurotransmission systems associated with the “positive reinforcing effects” of drugs of abuse, and other major structures related to brain stress systems and associated with the “negative reinforcing effects” of drug addiction [[63], [67]]. Thus, further studies could investigate the role of extended amygdala in the transitionality from use to abuse of drugs.
A new parallel addictive behavior
Over the past decades, the way of eating has changed dramatically. Among the historical changes that have characterized the last century, Western Countries are assisting to a set of changes in food culture, which have revealed a tendency to consume more frequently and more heavily those foodstuffs once considered rare and valuable. The prevailing tendency to eat more than necessary, often accompanied by significant imbalances between the various components of the diet, has led to a higher incidence of eating disorders (ED). More recently, the hypotheses that several of the same brain systems and neurotransmission circuits are involved in the rewarding effects related to foods and drugs has been suggested. It is conceivable the switching from the same neural systems in food and drugs [[68], [69], [70]], raising the hypotheses that binge-eating disorders could be considered as addictive behaviors. Here, we revised studies showing the possibility to study the key features of eating disorders, such as compulsive eating, with the paradigms used in drug addiction pre-clinical research.
The legitimacy of the term “food addiction”
Compulsive eating, is very similar to compulsive drug intake [78], and compulsive eating might be considered an “addiction” in its own right. Studies in humans and laboratory animals showed that, aside to energy balance, eating behavior is regulated by factors unrelated to metabolic control and data from clinical studies suggest that some over-eaters can develop addictive-like behaviors when they consume pleasurable foods [[26], [83]]. It has been proposed that overeating of palatable food may produce long-term neuroadaptations in the reward and stress networks of the brain [[10], [84]], similar to those produced by long-term drug abuse [26]. Taken together, these evidences suggest that compulsive eating, as well as compulsive drug-seeking can be explained using the same three major theories driving the experimental research on drug addiction, thus exploring the possibility of a sort of “transitionality” from a moderate use of pleasurable foodstuffs to their abuse.
Recent evidence from mice and monkeys suggest the possibility of producing animal models of eating disorders [[71], [72], [77], [85], [86], [87]]. It has been shown that rats with the possibility to assume a calorie-free saccharin solution or to self-administer intravenous cocaine infusions, they irrefutably chose the former solution rather than the second one [77]. This suggests how the macro-nutrients in pleasurable food can activate brain reward systems independently of their caloric load [78]. Furthermore, pleasurable foods can activate brain neurotransmission systems related to reward, motivation, and decision making [69]. Highly palatable foods induce long-lasting memories in non-human-primate models of chocolate preference [86], and the sudden food-reward absence induces anxiety-like behaviors (i.e., exploration), without changes in the levels of stress hormone cortisol [87]. Relying on these findings, eating behaviors related to the learning of food-associated cues seem to be important in the incidence and/or relapse of eating disorders. Finally, since the main features of drug addiction, such as compulsive seeking-behavior and relapse may be reproduced using several animal models, it can be considered the possibility to study food addiction using the animal models that previously defined the main features of drug addiction.
The neural basis of food addiction
The activation of the DA-containing link in brain reward circuitry is the most clear and superimposable defined in food and drug-seeking behavior [[25], [26], [69]]. Particularly, DA release seems to correlate with subjective reward from both drug and food use in humans [[25], [69]]. Repeated mesolimbic DA stimulation induced by expositions to addictive drugs produces brain plastic changes that result in compulsive drug-seeking. In a similar way, a repeated palatable foods exposition can induce compulsive food consumption using the same neurotransmission systems. Moreover, neuroimaging studies have revealed alterations in DA receptor expression in obese subjects that are similar to those found in drug-addicted subjects [[69], [78], [89], [90]].
Eating disorders are characterized by compulsive eating behavior, even in spite of dangerous circumstances. It has been hypothesized that a complex gene-environment interaction may be a key factor of compulsive eating behavior [[91], [92]]. Several studies have implicated DA type2 receptors (D2Rs) in the inclination to compulsive-like behaviors, as its happen in the drug addiction [[18], [93]]. Moreover, it has been demonstrate a gene-environment interaction in a mouse model compulsive chocolate-seeking/taking behavior using C57 and DBA mice in a conditioned suppression paradigm [[88], [94]]. In this study, we reproduced a compulsive eating behavior using the paradigm of conditioned suppression of a chocolate-seeking behavior [71] in order to compare the stressed C57 and DBA mice. Moreover, it has been hypothesized that low availability of accumbal D2Rs is considered a genetic risk factor in the incidence of food compulsive-seeking behavior and that the environment can induce a compulsive eating behavior altering the expression of D2Rs in the striatum. To this aim, we measured D1Rs and D2Rs expression in the striatum and D1Rs, D2Rs and NE-ergic α1 receptors (α1Rs) levels in the mpFC, respectively, by western blot [88]. We showed that exposure to a certain environmental condition (food restriction) inducing compulsive eating behavior, depends on genetic background, that is connected with a decreased availability of NAc D2Rs. Conversely, striatum D2Rs up-regulation and mpFC α1Rs down-regulation are induced during the compulsive eating behavior. These findings confirm the key role of a gene-environment interaction in the compulsive eating behavior, also supporting the idea that low availability of NAc D2Rs is a “constitutive” genetic risk factor for compulsive eating behavior. Finally, striatum D2R and mpFC α1R counteractive regulations are thought to be potential “neuroadaptive responses” paralleling with the transitionality from motivated to compulsive eating behaviors, and consequently in food addiction, as it has been hypothesized in drug addiction [[88], [94]].
Electrophysiological basis of food-directed behavior
Interestingly, it has been identified the presence of two neuronal types in the NAc [[102], [103]]: fast spiking interneurons (FSIs) and medium spiny neurons (MSNs). It has been reported that FSIs strongly inhibit MSNs, that exert a control their “spike timing” [[102], [104]], and that respond differently from MSNs to rewards [[102], [105]]. These findings suggest that FSIs and MSNs have different roles in those behaviors related to motivation and habit learning. Finally, the NAc plays an important role in the appetitive and consummatory behavior. Commonly, it was found that subpopulations of neurons in the NAc and VS respond phasically to every single characteristics of appetitive and consummatory phases [[97], [98], [99], [101]]. Since more NAc neurons are inhibited than excited during eating behavior, NAc inhibition manipulations may enhance food-seeking behavior. This not because of the general inactivation of the NAc, but because of the silencing of such neurons whose inhibit the food-seeking behavior. However, many of the same inhibited neurons driving motivated eating behavior are conversely excited during operant responding of environmental food-associated cues. It’s arguable if it is electrophysiologically possible to discriminate a dissociable role of mesolimbic structures of reward system in order to investigate a possible transitionality from a normal to compulsive eating behavior.
Conclusions
The first question is whether the three theoretical conceptualizations, the “incentive-salience theory”, the “hedonic dysregulation theory” and the “habit-based learning theory” are able to individually explain the psychopathological features of drug addiction. Alternatively, it is more likely that these three theories can be considered as parts of a unique general conceptualization that can better explain the psychopathological features of drug addiction. The hypothesis that an “aberrant motivation”, a “hedonic dysregulation”, and an “aberrant learning” can be sole features that can be included along a unique “temporal continuum” in the complex psychopathological drug-seeking/taking behavior should be considered.
The passage from occasional drug use to abuse is related to a change from a positive reinforcement to a negative one, with changes in motivational baseline [106]. Drug reward is comprised of two components: one appetitive (orienting towards food) and the other consummatory (hedonic evaluation), which are also referred to as “wanting” and “liking”, respectively. It has been explained that “wanting” and “liking” could act independently, defining a psychological and neuroanatomical separation between them [[2], [5]]. Moreover, it has been defined that craving (intense needing) and continuous neuroplastic changes are involved in the passage from use to abuse [11]. Furthermore, it has been argued that only maladaptive habit-based learning could trigger drug-seeking behavior [4]. However, these three hypotheses are able to explain singular features of the entire complex of drug addiction, such as the compulsive seeking behavior and the relapse. Alternatively, it is possible to consider a unique “temporal continuum” wherein (1) a progressively aberrant habit learning occurs during casual drug use, during which hedonic dysregulation is activated and (2) leads to a progressively aberrant “salience-incentivation” inducing the drug-taking behavior. Finally, motivation, hedonic dysregulation, and habit-based learning can be considered singular parts of a unique and complex drug-seeking/taking behavior; neuroanatomical and neurobiological evidence discussed here are in line with this hypotheses. However, although several studies have investigated how and when these three characteristics are involved in drug addiction, little is known about their possible juxtaposition in a single “temporal continuum”. Several human and animal studies have demonstrated that the time of reward has a strong role in reward processing [[22], [23]]. Furthermore, time windows and “reward rates” are of crucial importance for conditioning, and DA neurons are crucially involved in the processing of temporal information about the rewards. DA-ergic neurons in the meso-cortico-limbic system show predictive reward timing with a sensitivity induced by reward-related responses and by the instantaneity of reward probability [22]. This reinforces the hypotheses of a possible single “temporal continuum” from occasional use to compulsive use of substances, mediated by a meso-cortico-limbic DA-ergic circuit. At a clinical level, this would also help to understand how and when to intervene along the “temporal continuum” from occasional use to abuse of pharmacological substances, and to produce new therapeutic strategies in order to avoid the insurgency of the pathological drug-seeking/taking behavior. Moreover, it has been suggested that the so-called “extended amygdala circuit” may be delegated to act on the motivational, emotional, and habitual effects of drug addiction [[63], [64], [65], [66]]. Brain structures comprised in the extended amygdala have similarities in morphology, immunohistochemistry, and connectivity. Thus, further studies could investigate the role of extended amygdala in the transitionality from use to abuse of drugs [[107], [108], [109]].
A growing body of data hypothesizes the possibility of a behavioral/physiological overlap between drug and food addiction. A recent work of our group has hypothesized that mpFC Norepinephrine (NE) transmission also plays a key role in compulsive chocolate seeking/taking behavior, suggesting that mpFC NE has a role in motivated food seeking/taking behavior, regulated by mesolimbic DA-ergic transmission [71]. Moreover, it has been shown that mpFC NE enhances GABA-ergic neurotransmission via the α1 receptors [110], suggesting a crucial role of NE in the phenomenon of relapse in drug-seeking behavior [[111], [112], [113], [114], [115]]. Thus, further investigations about the role of NE in mediating interneuronal amygdaloid activity are strongly suggested, in order to better understand a possible meso-cortico-limbic pathway in the transitionality of both drug and food addiction [[116], [117], [118]].
The second question is whether the three features presented above (aberrant motivation, hedonic dysregulation and aberrant learning), and underlying drug-addicted behavior could also explain the psychopathological behavior that characterizes eating disorders. Although there are several studies about the behavioral/neurobiological overlap between drug and food addiction, little is known about the possible role of an “aberrant motivation”, a “hedonic dysregulation” and an “aberrant learning” in psychopathological behavior characterizing a possible transitionality in food addiction, from normal to compulsive eating behavior. These three theories could contribute to better understanding of the psychopathological features of eating disorders, such as the compulsive use and the relapse to the substances, which resemble characteristics of drug addiction. Thus, future works could aim to better understand the key elements characterizing the psycho-physio-pathological aspects of both drug and food addictions, such as the compulsive use and the relapse.
References
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. ; 2013 (Washington, DC)
- Berridge, K.C. Motivational concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Physiol Behav. 2004; 81: 179–209
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (421)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (1448)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (5)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (2019)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | Scopus (1)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (14)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (56)
- View in Article
- | Abstract
- | Full Text
- | Full Text PDF
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (436)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (88)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | Scopus (1538)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (0)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (187)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (459)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (5)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (447)
- View in Article
- | Abstract
- | Full Text
- | Full Text PDF
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (364)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (1143)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (2)
- View in Article
- | Abstract
- | Full Text
- | Full Text PDF
- | Scopus (15)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (561)
- View in Article
- | Abstract
- | Full Text
- | Full Text PDF
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (301)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (316)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (284)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (172)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (10)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (134)
- View in Article
- | Abstract
- | Full Text
- | Full Text PDF
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (224)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (339)
- View in Article
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (530)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (195)
- View in Article
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (44)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (1357)
- View in Article
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (658)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (95)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (187)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (794)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (274)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (88)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (441)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (153)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (102)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (326)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | Scopus (19)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (42)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (486)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (391)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (198)
- View in Article
- | Abstract
- | Full Text
- | Full Text PDF
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (314)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (134)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (60)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (148)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (29)
- View in Article
- | Abstract
- | Full Text
- | Full Text PDF
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (103)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (93)
- View in Article
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (30)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | Scopus (14)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (475)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (127)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (145)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (113)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (177)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (202)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (486)
- View in Article
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (37)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (375)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (26)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (98)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (39)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (3)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (1)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | Scopus (1)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (42)
- View in Article
- | Abstract
- | Full Text
- | Full Text PDF
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (198)
- View in Article
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (44)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (349)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | Scopus (4)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (86)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (67)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (31)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (32)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (5)
- View in Article
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (8)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (127)
- View in Article
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (26)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (36)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (101)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (28)
- View in Article
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (81)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (114)
- View in Article
- | PubMed
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (59)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (44)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (30)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (49)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (97)
- View in Article
- | Crossref
- | PubMed
- | Scopus (18)
- Koob, G.F. and Volkow, N.D. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35: 217–238DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.110
- Robbins, T.W. and Everitt, B.J. Introduction: the neurobiology of drug addiction: new vistas. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008; 363: 3109–3111DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0108
- Berridge, K.C. and Robinson, T.E. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience?. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998; 28: 309–369
- Kirkpatrick, M.G., Goldenson, N.I., Kapadia, N., Khaler, C.W., de Wit, H., Swift, R.M. et al. Emotional traits predict individual differences in amphetamine-induced positive mood in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2015; DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-015-4091-y
- Wardle, M.C. and de Wit, H. Effects of amphetamine on reactivity to emotional stimuli. Psychopharmacology. 2012; 220: 143–153DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2498-7
- Thomsen, K.R. Measuring anhedonia: impaired ability to pursue, experience, and learn about reward. Front Psychol. 2015; 6: 1409DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01409
- Koob, G.F. Animal models of craving for ethanol. Addiction. 2000; 95: S73–S81
- Parylak, S.L., Koob, G.F., and Zorrilla, E.P. The dark side of food addiction. Physiol Behav. 2011; 104: 149–156DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.063
- Koob, G.F. A role for brain stress systems in addiction. Neuron. 2008; 59: 11–34DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.012
- Gardner, E.L. Addiction and brain reward and antireward pathways. Adv Psychosom Med. 2011; 30: 22–60DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000324065
- Everitt, B.J. and Robbins, T.W. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci. 2005; 11: 1481–1487
- Alderson, H.L., Robbins, T.W., and Everitt, B.J. Heroin self-administration under a second-order schedule of reinforcement: acquisition and maintenance of heroin-seeking behaviour in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2000; 153: 120–133
- Arroyo, M., Markou, A., Robbins, T.W., and Everitt, B.J. Acquisition, maintenance and reinstatement of intravenous cocaine self-administration under a second-order schedule of reinforcement in rats: effects of conditioned cues and continuous access to cocaine. Psychopharmacology. 1998; 140: 331–344
- Everitt, B.J., Dickinson, A., and Robbins, T.W. The neuropsychological basis of addictive behaviour. Brain Res Rev. 2001; 36: 129–138
- Gasbarri, A., Pompili, A., Packard, M.G., and Tomaz, C. Habit learning and memory in mammals: behavioral and neural characteristics. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014; 114: 198–208DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2014.06.010
- Everitt, B.J., Belin, D., Economidou, D., Pelloux, Y., Dalley, J., and Robbins, T.W. Neural mechanisms underlying the vulnerability to develop compulsive drug-seeking habits and addiction. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008; 363: 3125–3135DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0089
- Dalley, J.W., Everitt, B.J., and Robbins, T.W. Impulsivity, compulsivity and top–down cognitive control. Neuron. 2011; 69: 680–694DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020
- Dickinson, A., Smith, S., and Mirenowicz, J. Dissociation of Pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning under dopamine antagonists. Behav Neurosci. 2000; 114: 468–483
- Cardinal, R.N., Parkinson, J.A., Hall, J., and Everitt, B.J. Emotion and motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002; 26: 321–352
- Bermudez, M.A. and Schultz, W. Timing in reward and decision processes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014; 369: 20120468DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0468
- Bermudez, M.A., Göbel, C., and Schultz, W. Sensitivity to temporal structure in amygdala neurons. Curr Biol. 2012; 9: 1839–1844DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.062
- Volkow, N.D. and Wise, R.A. How can drug addiction help us understand obesity?. Nat Neurosci. 2005; 8: 555–560
- Volkow, N.D., Wang, G.J., and Baler, R.D. Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: implications for obesity. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011; 15: 37–46DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.001
- Volkow, N.D., Wang, G.J., Fowler, J.S., and Telang, F. Overlapping neuronal circuits in addiction and obesity: evidence of systems pathology. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008; 363: 3191–3200DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0107
- Di Chiara, G. and Imperato, A. Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1988; 85: 5274–5278
- Wise, R.A. and Rompre, P.P. Brain dopamine and reward. Ann Rev Psychol. 1989; 40: 191–225
- Pontieri, F.E., Tanda, G., and Di Chiara, G. Intravenous cocaine, morphine and amphetaemine preferentially increase extracellular dopamine in the “shell” as compared with the “core” of the rat nucleus accumbens. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1995; 92: 12304–12308
- Bassareo, V. and Di Chiara, G. Differential responsiveness of dopamine transmission to food-stimuli in nucleus accumbens shell/core compartments. Neuroscience. 1999; 89: 637–641
- Pecina, S., Smith, K.S., and Berridge, K.C. Hedonic hot spots in the brain. Neuroscientist. 2006; 12: 500–511
- Puglisi-Allegra, S. and Ventura, R. Prefrontal/accumbal catecholamine system processes high motivational salience. Front Behav Neurosci. 2012; 6: 31DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00031
- Wang, G.J., Volkow, N.D., and Fowler, J.S. The role of dopamine in motivation for food in humans: implications for obesity. Expert Opin Ther Targets. 2002; 6: 601–609
- McClure, S.M., Daw, N.D., and Montague, P.R. A computational substrate for incentive salience. Trends Neurosci. 2003; 26: 423–428
- Jay, T.M. Dopamine: a potential substrate for synaptic plasticity and memory mechanisms. Prog Neurobiol. 2003; 69: 375–390
- Schultz, W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1998; 80: 1–27
- Kelley, A.E. Ventral striatal control of appetitive motivation: role in ingestive behavior and reward-related learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004; 27: 765–776
- Di Ciano, P. and Everitt, B.J. Dissociable effects of antagonism of NMDA and AMPA/KA receptors in the nucleus accumbens core and shell on cocaine-seeking behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001; 25: 341–360
- Sellings, L.H. and Clarke, P.B. Segregation of amphetamine reward and locomotor stimulation between nucleus accumbens medial shell and core. J Neurosci. 2003; 23: 6295–6303
- Ito, R., Dalley, J.W., Howes, S.R., Robbins, T.W., and Everitt, B.J. Dissociation in conditioned dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens core and shell in response to cocaine cues and during cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. J Neurosci. 2000; 20: 7489–7495
- Cheng, J.J., de Bruin, J.P., and Feenstra, M.G. Dopamine efflux in nucleus accumbens shell and core in response to appetitive classical conditioning. Eur J Neurosci. 2003; 18: 1306–1314
- Kalivas, P.W. and Volkow, N.D. The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry. 2005; 162: 1403–1413
- Haber, S.N., Fudge, J.L., and McFarland, N.R. Striatonigrostriatal pathways in primates form an ascending spiral from the shell to the dorsolateral striatum. J Neurosci. 2000; 20: 2369–2382
- Haber, S.N. The primate basal ganglia: parallel and integrative networks. J Chem Neuroanat. 2003; 26: 317–330
- Parkinson, J.A., Cardinal, R.N., and Everitt, B.J. Limbic cortical-ventral striatal systems underlying appetitive conditioning. Prog Brain Res. 2000; 126: 263–285
- Di Ciano, P. and Everitt, B.J. Direct interactions between baso-lateral amygdala and nucleus accumbens core underlie cocaine-seeking behavior by rats. J Neurosci. 2004; 24: 7167–7173
- Hyman, S.E. and Malenka, R.C. Addiction and the brain: the neurobiology of compulsion and its persistence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001; 2: 695–703
- Corbit, L.H. and Balleine, B.W. Double dissociation of basolateral and central amygdala lesions on the general and outcome-specific forms of pavlovian-instrumental transfer. J Neurosci. 2005; 25: 962–970
- Tomaz, C., Dickinson-Anson, H., and McGaugh, J.L. Basolateral amygdala lesions block diazepam-induced anterograde amnesia in an inhibitory avoidance task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1992; 15: 3615–3619
- Tomaz, C., Dickinson-Anson, H., McGaugh, J.L., Souza-Silva, M.A., Viana, M.B., and Graeff, E.G. Localization in the amygdala of the amnestic action of diazepam on emotional memory. Behav Brain Res. 1993; 58: 99–105
- Milton, A.L., Lee, J.L., and Everitt, B.J. Reconsolidation of appetitive memories for both natural and drug reinforcement is dependent on β-adrenergic receptors. Learn Mem. 2008; 15: 88–92DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.825008
- Paton, J.J., Belova, M.A., Morrison, S.E., and Salzman, C.D. The primate amygdala represents the positive and negative value of visual stimuli during learning. Nature. 2006; 439: 865–870
- See, R.E., Kruzich, P.J., and Grimm, J.W. Dopamine, but not glutamate, receptor blockade in the basolateral amygdala attenuates conditioned reward in a rat model of relapse to cocaine-seeking behavior. Psychopharmacology. 2001; 154: 301–310
- Neisewander, J.L., O’Dell, L.E., Tran-Nguyen, L.T., Castaňeda, E., and Fuchs, R.A. Dopamine overflow in the nucleus accumbens during extinction and reinstatement of cocaine self-administration behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1996; 15: 506–514
- McFarland, K., Davidge, S.B., Lapish, C.C., and Kalivas, P.W. Limbic and motor circuitry underlying footshock-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior. J Neurosci. 2004; 24: 1551–1560
- Parsegian, A. and See, R.E. Dysregulation of dopamine and glutamate release in the prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens following methamphetamine self-administration and during reinstatement in rats. J Neurosci. 2014; 27: 2045–2057DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2013.231
- Belin, D., Belin-Rauscent, A., Murray, J.E., and Everitt, B.J. Addiction: failure of control over maladaptive incentive habits. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013; 23: 564–572DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.025
- Bechara, A., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A.R. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000; 10: 295–307
- Yin, H.H., Knowlton, B.J., and Balleine, B.W. Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur J Neurosci. 2004; 19: 181–189
- Yin, H.H., Ostlund, S.B., Knowlton, B.J., and Balleine, B.W. The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning. Eur J Neurosci. 2005; 22: 513–523
- Faure, A., Haberland, U., Conde, F., and El Massioui, N. Lesion to the nigrostriatal dopamine system disrupts stimulus-response habit formation. J Neurosci. 2005; 25: 2771–2780
- Belin, D. and Everitt, B.J. Cocaine seeking habits depend upon dopamine-dependent serial connectivity linking the ventral with the dorsal striatum. Neuron. 2008; 57: 432–441DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.019
- Koob, G.F. Brain stress systems in the amygdala and addiction. Brain Res. 2009; 1293: 61–75DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.03.038
- Koob, G.F. Addiction is a reward deficit and stress surfeit disorder. Front Psychiatr. 2013; 4: 72DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00072
- Jennings, J.H., Sparta, D.R., Stamatakis, A.M., Ung, R.L., Pleil, K.E., Kash, T.L. et al. Distinct extended amygdala circuits for divergent motivational states. Nature. 2013; 496: 224–228DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature12041
- Stamatakis, A.M., Sparta, D.R., Jennings, J.H., McElligott, Z.A., Decot, H., and Stuber, G.D. Amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis circuitry: implications for addiction-related behaviors. Neuropharmacology. 2014; 76: 320–328DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.046
- LeMoal, M. and Koob, G.F. Drug addiction: pathways to the disease and pathophysiological perspectives. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007; 17: 377–393
- Ventura, R., Morrone, C., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. Prefrontal/accumbal catecholamine system determines motivational salience attribution to both reward- and aversion-related stimuli. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007; 104: 5181–5186
- Kelley, A.E. and Berridge, K.C. The neuroscience of natural rewards: relevance to addictive drugs. J Neurosci. 2002; 22: 3306–3311
- Berner, L.A., Bocarsly, M.E., Hoebel, B.G., and Avena, N.M. Baclofen suppresses binge eating of pure fat but not a sugar-rich or sweet-fat diet. Behav Pharmacol. 2009; 20: 631–634DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0b013e328331ba47
- Latagliata, E.C., Patrono, E., Puglisi-Allegra, S., and Ventura, R. Food seeking in spite of harmful consequences is under prefrontal cortical noradrenergic control. BMC Neurosci. 2010; 8: 11–15DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-11-15
- Avena, N.M., Rada, P., and Hoebel, B.G. Evidence for sugar addiction: behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008; 32: 20–39
- Bancroft, J. and Vukadinovic, Z. Sexual addiction, sexual compulsivity, sexual impulsivity, or what? Toward a theoretical model. J Sex Res. 2004; 41: 225–234
- Petry, N.M. Should the scope of addictive behaviors be broadened to include pathological gambling?. Addiction. 2006; 101: 152–160
- Ziauddeen, H., Farooqi, I.S., and Fletcher, P.C. Obesity and the brain: how convincing is the addiction model?. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012; 13: 279–286DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3212
- Avena, N.M., Rada, P., Moise, N., and Hoebel, B.G. Sucrose sham feeding on a binge schedule releases accumbens dopamine repeatedly and eliminates the acetylcholine satiety response. Neuroscience. 2006; 139: 813–820
- Lenoir, M., Serre, F., Cantin, L., and Ahmed, S. Intense sweetness surpasses cocaine reward. PLoS ONE. 2007; 2: e698
- Wang, G.J., Volkow, N.D., Telang, F., Jayne, M., Ma, J., Rao, M. et al. Exposure to appetitive food stimuli markedly activates the human brain. Neuroimage. 2004; 21: 1790–1797
- Deroche-Gamonet, V., Belin, D., and Piazza, P.V. Evidence for addiction-like behavior in the rat. Science. 2004; 305: 1014–1017
- Gilpin, N.W. and Koob, G.F. Neurobiology of alcohol dependence: focus on motivational mechanisms. Alcohol Res Health. 2008; 31: 185–195
- Gilpin, N.W. and Koob, G.F. Effects of β-adrenoceptor antagonists on alcohol drinking by alcohol-dependent rats. Psychopharmacology. 2010; 212: 431–439DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-1967-8
- Vanderschuren, L.J. and Everitt, B.J. Drug seeking becomes compulsive after prolonged cocaine self-administration. Science. 2004; 305: 1017–1019
- Heyne, A., Kiesselbach, C., and Sahùn, I. An animal model of compulsive food-taking behaviour. Addict Biol. 2009; 14: 373–383DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00175.x
- Corwin, R.L., Avena, N.M., and Boggiano, M.M. Feeding and reward: perspectives from three rat models of binge eating. Physiol Behav. 2011; 104: 87–97DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.041
- LeMerrer, J. and Stephens, D.N. Food-induced behavioral sensitization, its cross-sensitization to cocaine and morphine, pharmacological blockade, and effect on food intake. J Neurosci. 2006; 26: 7163–7171
- Duarte, R.B.M., Patrono, E., Borges, A.C., César, A.A.S., Tomaz, C., Ventura, R. et al. Consumption of a highly palatable food induces a lasting place-conditioning memory in marmoset monkeys. Behav Process. 2014; 107: 163–166DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.08.021
- Duarte, R.B.M., Patrono, E., Borges, A.C., Tomaz, C., Ventura, R., Gasbarri, A. et al. High versus low fat/sugar food affects the behavioral, but not the cortisol response of marmoset monkeys in a conditioned-place-preference task. Physiol Behav. 2015; 139: 442–448DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.065
- Patrono, E., Di Segni, M., Patella, L., Andolina, D., Valzania, A., Latagliata, E.C. et al. When chocolate seeking becomes compulsion: gene-environment interplay. PLoS ONE. 2015; 10: e0120191DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120191
- Hoebel, B.G., Avena, N.M., Bocarsly, M.E., and Rada, P. Natural addiction: a behavioral and circuit model based on sugar addiction in rats. J Addict Med. 2009; 3: 33–41DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ADM.0b013e31819aa621
- Kenny, P.J. Reward mechanisms in obesity: new insights and future directions. Neuron. 2011; 69: 664–679DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.016
- Bulik, C.M. Exploring the gene-environment nexus in eating disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2005; 30: 335–339
- Campbell, I.C., Mill, J., Uher, R., and Schmidt, U. Eating disorders, gene-environment interactions and epi-genetics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010; 35: 784–793DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.09.012
- Volkow, N.D., Fowler, J.S., Wang, G.J., Baler, R., and Telang, F. Imaging dopamine’s role in drug abuse and addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2009; 56: 3–8DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.05.022
- Di Segni, M., Patrono, E., Patella, L., Puglisi-Allegra, S., and Ventura, R. Animal models of compulsive eating behavior. Nutrients. 2015; 6: 4591–4609DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/nu6104591
- Berke, J.D. Fast oscillations in cortical-striatal networks switch frequency following rewarding events and stimulant drugs. Eur J Neurosci. 2009; 30: 848–859DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06843.x
- Ren, X., Ferreira, J.G., Zhou, L., Shammah-Lagnado, S.J., Jeckel, C.W., and de Araujo, I.E. Nutrient selection in the absence of taste receptor signaling. J Neurosci. 2010; 30: 8012–8023DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5749-09.2010
- Wiltschko, A.B., Pettibone, J.R., and Berke, J.D. Opposite effects of stimulant and antipsychotic drugs on striatal fast-spiking interneurons. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35: 1261–1270DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.226
- Cacciapaglia, F., Wightman, R.M., and Carelli, R.M. Rapid dopamine signaling differentially modulates distinct microcircuits within the nucleus accumbens during sucrose-directed behavior. J Neurosci. 2011; 31: 13860–13869DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1340-11.2011
- Shimura, T., Imaoka, H., Okazaki, Y., Kanamori, Y., Fushiki, T., and Yamamoto, T. Involvement of the mesolimbic system in palatability-induced ingestion. Chem Senses. 2005; 30: i188–i189
- Nishijo, H., Uwano, T., Tamura, R., and Ono, T. Gustatory and multimodal responses in the amygdala during licking and discrimination of sensory stimuli in awake rats. J Neurophysiol. 1998; 79: 21–36
- Nishijo, H., Uwano, T., and Ono, T. Representation of taste stimuli in the brain. Chem Senses. 2005; 30: i174–i175
- Matsumoto, J., Urakawa, S., Hori, E., de Araujo, M.F., Sakuma, Y., Ono, T. et al. Neuronal responses in the nucleus accumbens shell during sexual behavior in male rats. J Neurosci. 2012; 32: 1672–1686DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5140-11.2012
- Meredith, G.E. The synaptic framework for chemical signaling in nucleus accumbens. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999; 877: 140–156
- Tepper, J.M. and Plenz, D. Microcircuits in the striatum: striatal cell types and their interaction. in: S. Grillner, A.M. Graybiel (Eds.) Microcircuits: the interface between neurons and global brain function. MIT, Cambridge; 2006: 127–148
- Lansink, C.S., Goltstein, P.M., Lankelma, J.V., and Pennartz, C.M. Fast-spiking interneurons of the rat ventral striatum: temporal coordination of activity with principal cells and responsiveness to reward. Eur J Neurosci. 2010; 32: 494–508DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07293.x
- Piazza, P.V. and Deroche-Gamonet, V. A multistep general theory of transition to addiction. Psychopharmacology. 2013; 229: 387–413DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-013-3224-4
- Greba, Q., Gifkins, A., and Kokkinidis, L. Inhibition of amygdaloid dopamine D2 receptors impairs emotional learning measured with fear-potentiated startle. Brain Res. 2001; 899: 218–226
- Guarraci, F.A., Frohardt, R.J., Young, S.L., and Kapp, B.S. A functional role for dopamine transmission in the amygdala during conditioned fear. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999; 877: 732–736
- Rosenkranz, J.A. and Grace, A.A. Cellular mechanisms of infralimbic and prelimbic prefrontal cortical inhibition and dopaminergic modulation of basolateral amygdala neurons in vivo. J Neurosci. 2002; 22: 324–337
- Dumont, E.C. and Williams, J.T. Noradrenaline triggers GABAA inhibition of bed nucleus of the stria terminalis neurons projecting to the ventral tegmental area. J Neurosci. 2004; 24: 8198–8204
- Smith, R.J. and Aston-Jones, G. Noradrenergic transmission in the extended amygdala: role in increased drug-seeking and relapse during protracted drug abstinence. Brain Struct Funct. 2008; 213: 43–61DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-008-0191-3
- Ventura, R., Cabib, S., Alcaro, A., Orsini, C., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. Norepinephrine in the prefrontal cortex is critical for amphetamine-induced reward and mesoaccumbens dopamine release. J Neurosci. 2003; 23: 1879–1885
- Ventura, R., Alcaro, A., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. Prefrontal cortical norepinephrine release is critical for morphine-induced reward, reinstatement and dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens. Cereb Cortex. 2005; 15: 1877–1886
- van der Meulen, J.A., Joosten, R.N., de Bruin, J.P., and Feenstra, M.G. Dopamine and noradrenaline efflux in the medial prefrontal cortex during serial reversals and extinction of instrumental goal-directed behavior. Cereb Cortex. 2007; 17: 1444–1453
- Mitrano, D.A., Schroeder, J.P., Smith, Y., Cortright, J.J., Bubula, N., Vezina, P. et al. α-1 adrenergic receptors are localized on presynaptic elements in the nucleus accumbens and regulate mesolimbic dopamine transmission. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012; 37: 2161–2172DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2012.68
- Stevenson, C.W. and Gratton, A. Basolateral amygdala modulation of the nucleus accumbens dopamine response to stress: role of the medial prefrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2003; 17: 1287–1295
- Floresco, S.B. and Tse, M.T. Dopaminergic regulation of inhibitory and excitatory transmission in the basolateral amygdala-prefrontal cortical pathway. J Neurosci. 2007; 27: 2045–2057
- Ito, R. and Canseliet, M. Amphetamine exposure selectively enhances hippocampus-dependent spatial learning and attenuates amygdala-dependent cue learning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35: 1440–1452DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.14