J Neurother. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 Nov 3.
Published in final edited form as:
J Neurother. 2008 Dec 1; 12(4): 205–225.
Estate Sokhadze, Ph.D., Shraddha Singh, Ph.D., Christopher Stewart, M.D., Michael Hollifield, M.D., Ayman El-Baz, Ph.D., and Allan Tasman, M.D.
See other articles in PMC that cite the published article.
Abstract
Cocaine addiction places a specific burden on mental health services through its comorbidity with other psychiatric disorders. Treatment of patients with cocaine abuse is more complicated when addiction is co-occurring with PTSD. This study used dense-array event-related potential (ERP) technique to investigate whether the patients with this form of dual diagnosis display excessive reactivity to both trauma and drug cues as compared to neutral cues. Cue reactivity refers to a phenomenon in which individuals with a history of drug dependence exhibit verbal, physiological, and behavioral responses to cues associated with their preferred substance of abuse. This study explores ERP differences associated with cue-related responses to both drug and trauma cues in a three-category oddball task using neutral, drug-, and trauma-related pictorial stimuli. The study was conducted on 14 cocaine dependent subjects, 11 subjects with cocaine dependence comorbid with PTSD, and 9 age- and gender-matched control subjects. A 128 channel Electrical Geodesics EEG system was used to record ERP during the visual three-category oddball task with three categories (neutral, drug, stress) of affective pictures. Patients with cocaine dependence and PTSD, as compared to patients with only cocaine addiction and control subjects, showed excessive cue reactivity to both drug- and trauma-related visual stimuli. Most profound differences were found in the amplitude and latency of frontal P3a, and centro-parietal P3b ERP components. Group differences were found as well between patients with cocaine abuse (both addiction-only and dual diagnosis groups) vs. controls on most ERP measures for drug-related cues. We propose that the employed ERP cue reactivity variables could be used as valuable functional outcome measures in dually diagnosed drug addicts undergoing behavioral treatment.
INTRODUCTION
Comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), which is highly prevalent among cocaine abusers, is known to be associated with poorer treatment outcomes due to aggravation of factors contributing to cocaine addiction development.
Cocaine addicts with co-occurring PTSD have a more persistent illness course and are more refractive to treatment than those without dual diagnosis (Brown & Wolfe, 1995; Brown et al., 1995; Coffey et al., 2002; Evans & Sullivan, 2001; O’Brien et al., 2004). In dually diagnosed patients, symptoms of both disorders are in complex relationships where one disorder serves to sustain another (Chilcoat & Breslau, 1998; Jacobsen, Southwick, & Kosten, 2001; Saladin et al., 2003; Shiperd et al., 2005)
There are different approaches to explain high rate of co-occurrence of PTSD and cocaine addiction (Stewart et al., 1998), including those based on concepts from the cognitive neuroscience field (Sokhadze et al., 2007). Preoccupation with drug and drug-related items is a typical characteristic of addicted individuals. Several research studies provided support for the hypothesis that the process of alteration of attention takes place in addicts (Hester, Dixon, & Garavan, 2006; Lyvers, 2000; Robinson & Berridge, 2003), so called “attentional bias”(Franken et al., 1999,2000; Franken, 2003), and drug-related cues attain greater salience and motivational significance (Cox et a., 2006) Cue reactivity refers to a phenomenon in which individuals with a drug dependence exhibit excessive verbal, physiological, and behavioral responses to cues associated with their preferred substance of abuse (Carter & Tiffany, 1999; Childress et al., 1999; Drummond et al., 1995). Furthermore, in cocaine abusers cue reactivity has been shown to be dependent on cue type and modality (Johnson et al., 1998). One of the cognitive components of cue reactivity in substance abusers is the preferential allocation of attentional resources for items related to drug use (Lubman et al., 2000) or to alcohol use (Stormak et al., 2000). It has been proposed that conditioned sensitization in neural pathways associating incentives with stimulus items may be responsible for cue reactivity (Franken, 2003; Weiss et al., 2001).
Several neuroimaging studies have reported effects associated with drug cue-related responses and craving in cocaine addiction (Childress et al., 1999; Garavan et al., 2000; Hester et al., 2006; Kilts et al., 2001,2004). PTSD in persons with cocaine abuse is associated with more severe drug dependence, and on the other hand, the neurotoxiceffects of cocaine abuse can aggravate PTSD (Brown et al., 1995; Najavits et al., 1998;; Ouimette et al., 1997,1999). Only a few studies have examined mechanisms by which PTSD might exert an adverse effects on the course of addiction (Ouimette & Brown, 2003; Stewart et al., 1998). In substance use disorder (SUD) and PTSD comorbidity research, one of the main challenges is to obtain knowledge of cognitive processes that correlate with both cue reactivity and PTSD symptoms.
It has been shown that emotional abnormalities are typical for addicts (Fukunishi, 1996; Handelsman, et al., 2000). Addicted individuals could be affected by a dysregulation associated with changes in emotional reactivity to natural positive reinforcers (Volkow et al., 2003). Sensitization to drugs and counter-adaptation are hypothesized to contribute to dysregulation of both hedonic homeostasis and observed brain reward abnormalities (Koob, 1997; Koob & Le Moal, 1999; Koob et al., 2004). Emotional disturbances are also common for patients with PTSD. Physiological reactivity on exposure to internal or external cues that symbolize or resemble an aspect of the traumatic event is a core feature of PTSD (APA, 2000; Vasterling & Brewin, 2005). Research findings have consistently demonstrated that individuals with PTSD produce heightened physiological responses (e.g., startle, heart rate, skin conductance response, etc.) to stimuli related to traumatic events (Blanchard, 1990; Shalev et al., 1993; Orr & Roth, 2000; Prins et al., 1995). This heightened arousal has been found across a variety of psychophysiological measures during presentation of trauma-related auditory or visual cues, and during personal imagery of traumatic events (Blanchard et al., 1993; Casada et al., 1998; Orr et al., 1998; Sahar et al., 2001). Because physiological reactivity on exposure to cues related to traumatic events is common for PTSD, physiological assessments using electroencephalographic (EEG) measures such as event-related potentials (ERP) in PTSD co-occurring with cocaine addiction can provide valuable practical and theoretical insight.
The P300 component (300 to 600 ms post-stimulus) is the most widely used ERP measure in psychiatric and other clinical applications (Polich & Herbst, 200; Pritchard, 1981,1986; Pritchard, Sokhadze, & Houlihan, 2001). The amplitude of P300 reflects the allocation of attentional resources, while the latency is considered to reflect stimulus evaluation and classification time (Katayama & Polich, 1996; Polich et al., 1994). The P300 is usually obtained in oddball paradigm, wherein two stimuli are presented in a random order, one of them frequent, (standard) and another one rare (target) (Polich, 1990). A modification of the oddball task has been used where a third, also rare stimulus (distracter) is presented along with standard and target stimuli. It was reported that these infrequent distracters elicit a fronto-central P300, so called P3a, whereas the rare targets elicit a centro-parietal P300, so called P3b (Katayama & Polich, 1998). The P3a is recorded at the anterior scalp locations and has been interpreted as reflecting frontal lobe activity (Friedman et al., 1993; Knight, 1984). Whereas the P300 in general is thought to represent “context updating/closure” (Donchin & Coles, 1988), in three-stimuli oddball task the P3a is interpreted as “orienting”, and the P3b as an index of an ability to maintain sustained attention to target (Naatanen, 1990; Potts et al., 2004; Wijers et al., 1996). The anterior P3a indexes the contextual salience of the rare stimuli, whereas posterior P3b is indexing task-relevance of the stimuli (Gaeta, Friedman, & Hunt, 2003). The three-stimulus category oddball paradigm provides possibilities for delineating the cognitive processes engaged in this task when motivational salience of novel distracter stimuli are manipulated.
Most of the studies on PTSD report abnormalities in P300, which provide presumptive evidence for impaired cognitive processing in this disorder (Attias et al., 1996; Blomhoff et al., 1998;Charles et al., 1995; Felmingham et al., 2002; Karl, Malta, & Maerker, 2006; Kimble et al., 2000; Stanford et al., 2001). Studies finding attenuated P300 attribute their results to concentration impairment (McFarlane, Weber, & Clark, 1993), or attention deficits (Charles et al., 1995; Metzger et al., 1997a,b). Increased P300 amplitude was explained as due to altered selective attention (Attias et al., 1996), or heightened orientation to threatening stimuli (Kimble et al., 2000). Several studies emphasize that P3a enhancement in PTSD is expressed when distracters are either trauma-related or novel stimuli in oddball tasks (Bleich, Attias, & Furman, 1996; Drake et al., 1991; Felmingham et al., 2002; Weinstein, 1995). Increased P300 amplitude in PTSD is thought to reflect attentional bias towards threat stimuli and reduced P300 amplitude is thought to reflect a consequent reduction in attentional resources to non-threatening stimuli.
Acute and chronic use of cocaine exerts neuropharmacological effects on amplitude and latency of ERPs (Bauer, 1997; Biggins et al., 1997; Fein, Biggins, & MacKay, 1996; Kouiri et al., 1996). Longer P300 latency without abnormalities in amplitude has been reported in several studies on cocaine withdrawal (Bauer & Kranzler, 1994; Herning, Glover, Guo, 1994; Noldy, & Carlen, 1997). The majority of ERP studies aimed to assess cortical dysfunctions have used P3b tasks, and there are only few studies of P3a in addiction. Understanding contribution of frontal ERP components is important considering increased evidence of frontal dysfunctions in drug abuse, and specifically in cocaine abuse (Hester & Garavan, 2004)
According to the attentional biasing concept, patients with cocaine addiction with co-occurring PTSD in an attention task with pictorial emotional stimuli are expected to show enhanced reactivity to both cocaine- and traumatic stress-related cues due to preferential processing of drug- and trauma distracters; and consequently are expected to present lowered attentional resources availability for the processing of task-relevant target signals. The specific aim of this study is to examine cue reactivity to drug- and trauma-associated stimuli in a modification of cue reactivity test in three groups:dual diagnosis of cocaine dependence and PTSD (DUAL), cocaine addiction without PTSD (SUD), and controls (CNT). In this experiment we use an oddball task with distracters being either drug- , traumatic stress- related or emotionally neutral pictorial cue. Our aim is to examine as well drug- and trauma-related cues interference on both behavioral performance and cognitive ERP P300 (P3a, P3b) indices. By using both drug-related and trauma related cues to create interference we attempt to address the question of how both categories of cues may affect performance on task of the three study groups by assessing behavioral (reaction time, accuracy) and ERP indices (P3a, P3b), We predicted preferential selective attention to drug-related items but not to traumatic stress images in the SUD group, and enhanced processing of both drug- and trauma-related distracters in the DUAL group . Processing of highly salient but task-irrelevant distracters was expected to result in a decreased attentional capacity and a reduced allocation of resources to process task-relevant targets. This effect was predicted to be manifested in a delayed reaction time (RT), lower accuracy, lower magnitude of posterior ERP indices of task-relevant information processing (P3b) in DUAL patients compared to SUD and CNT groups. Thus, the goal of the study was to examine ERP measures of cue reactivity to drug- and trauma-associated stimuli, and to investigate how heightened orienting to these salient distracters will interfere with cognitive functions during performance on a visual three-category oddball task. We predicted an increased amplitude of the anterior ERP component (e.g., P3a) in response to novel pictorial distracters containing both drug- and trauma-related cues, and a reduced posterior ERP (e.g., P3b) in response to neutral targets and frequent standards in the DUAL group compared to the other groups. We expected that the patients with cocaine dependence and PTSD diagnoses compared to controls will show enhanced reactivity to the task-irrelevant drug- and threat-related cues, and will present selective attention to these highly motivationally salient signals which will negatively affect processing of task-relevant stimuli.
METHODS
Subjects
Cocaine abusing/dependent subjects were referred primarily from the University of Louisville Hospital emergency rooms, drug abuse treatment outpatient services, such as Jefferson County Alcohol and Drug Abuse Center (JADAC), and other psychiatric ambulatory units. There are established collaborations with other facilities and Louisville metro agencies. Dr. Stewart, a co-investigator in this study, is a Medical Director at JADAC and a clinical consultant at two residential addiction treatment centers (The Healing Place and Volunteers of America) located in Louisville metro area. He provided a substantial number of referrals through these programs. Dr. Hollifield, another co-investigator in the study, is a Director of the Anxiety Disorder Program at the University of Louisville, and consulted on diagnosis of PTSD in addicted patients from the pool of referred patients with cocaine addiction. Participating subjects were provided with full information about the study including the purpose, requirements, responsibilities, reimbursement, risks, benefits, alternatives, and role of the local Institutional Review Board (IRB). The consent forms were reviewed and explained to all subjects who expressed interest to participate. All questions were answered before consent signature was requested. If the individual agreed to participate, she/he signed and dated the consent form and received a copy countersigned by the investigator who obtained consent.
All procedures were conducted within the facilities of the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science and the University of Louisville Hospital. Initial contact with prospective participant was typically made via the telephone screening. An interviewer queried callers regarding major study criteria. Those meeting criteria received an appointment for consent, typically within 1 to 5 days after their initial call. Control subjects in this study were recruited from the Louisville metro community by advertisements approved by the local IRB. Responders were telephone screened to meet initial inclusion criteria. All control subjects were free of neurological or significant medical disorders, had normal hearing and vision, and were free of psychiatric disorders. Following telephone screening, the control subjects received a psychiatric assessment in the laboratory to verify the telephone screening and rule out Axis I diagnoses using Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First et al., 2001). Control subjects were chosen so that the control group was not significantly different from the patient group on age, education level, handedness, sex, and ethnicity. The same consent procedures followed for the patients was applied to the controls. Since subjects were participating in research, they were paid for their time. Payment methods followed the University of Louisville Health Science Center’s Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects’ guidelines concerning reimbursement for research time and parking. Participants were paid $20/hour for completing required research activities (e.g., ERP tests, providing urine sample, completing self-report forms) at each visit.
Psychiatric status questionnaires, drug use and psychosocial functioning screening
The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID I) (First et al., 2001) was used for Axis I diagnoses. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) was assessed using The Post-traumatic Symptom Scale-Self Report (PSS-SR) (Foa et al., 1989, 1997) questionnaire. The Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25 (HSCL-25) (Derogatis et al., 1974) was used to measure symptoms of anxiety and depression. Handedness of patients was assessed using the Edinburgh inventory (Oldfield,1971). Scores from the Addiction Severity Index (ASI) were used to measure problem severity in the areas of medical, employment, drug abuse, legal, family, social, and psychiatric difficulties (McLellan et al., 1980). Cocaine Negative Consequences Checklist (Michalec et al., 1996) was used to assess short-term and long-term adverse effects resulting from cocaine use. Psychosocial adjustment were assessed using the Social Adjustment Scale (SAS) (Weissman & Bothwell, 1976).
Qualitative urine toxicology screens (DrugCheck 4, NxStep, Amedica Biotech Inc., CA) were conducted in each subject to confirm cocaine abuse. In addition, qualitative urine toxicology screens for amphetamines, opiates and marijuana were performed to assess presence of additional abused substances (e.g., amphetamine, opiates, marijuana). Positive test for marijuana was not considered as exclusion criteria. Qualitative Saliva drug test (ALCO SCREEN, Chematics, Inc., IN) was also used to rule out current alcohol use.
Subjects in the study
Twenty five cocaine abusing/dependent subjects (9 females, 16 males) mean age, 41.3 ±6.1, range 32-52 years, 64% Afro-Americans) participated in the study. Fourteen of them were cocaine abusing subjects without PTSD and they were assigned to SUD group (42.2 ± 6.6 year-olds, 6 females, 8 males), whereas eleven cocaine addicts were diagnosed with PTSD (diagnosis was confirmed by consensus of Drs. Stewart and Hollifield) and comprised dually (SUD-PTSD) diagnosed group (DUAL). Six of them have been already earlier diagnosed with PTSD and had record of PTSD in their history at the intake stage. Dual group consisted of 3 females and 8 males (38.8 ± 6.3 years). Nine non-drug using control subjects (4 females, mean age, 36.7 ± 5.3, range, 29 – 45 years, 44% Afro-Americans) (CNT group) also participated in this study.
Twelve subjects in SUD group tested positive for cocaine, and 7 of them as well tested positive for marijuana use. Two subjects in SUD who did not tested positive were recovering addicts enrolled in this study after the inpatient JADAC rehabilitation course with abstinence period of less than 60 days. Nine subjects in the DUAL group tested positive for cocaine use, five of them also tested positive for marijuana use. Therefore the majority of our outpatient population consisted of current cocaine users, with almost the half of them using marijuana as a drug of second choice. Most preferred form of administration of the drug was smoking crack cocaine. Only one subject from cocaine addicts in this study used cocaine intravenously. Majority of addicted subjects reported regular use of nicotine/smoking. None of the subjects in SUD group were in any treatment program other than participating in Narcotics Anonymous (NA) or Alcoholic Anonymous (AA) meetings. All of the subjects except 2 patients from SUD group, one from DUAL group, and one from CNT group were right-handed. All control participants reported no current or past history of neurological or psychiatric disorders or dependence on any substances other than nicotine or caffeine. Subjects were fully informed about the nature of this research and signed informed consent form approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Louisville (Protocol IRB #240.06, pt. 2). For the specimen collection (urine drug screen) subjects signed a separate consent form also approved by the IRB within the same study protocol.
Stimulus presentation, EEG/ERP data acquisition and signal processing
All stimulus presentation, behavioral and subjective response collection was controlled by a computer running E-prime software (Psychology Software Tools, PA). Visual stimuli were presented on a 15″ flat-panel display. Manual responses were collected with a 5-button keypad. Subjects were instructed to press key number 1 when they see a picture of target category, and do not press key to non-target category images. In all experiments subjects were seated in a chair with their chin in a chinrest. The chinrest was placed so that subject’s eyes were 50 cm from the center of the flat panel screen. Breaks were provided every 10 minutes. All EEG data were acquired with a 128 channel Electrical Geodesics system (Net Station 200, v. 4.0) (Electrical Geodesics Inc., OR) running on a Macintosh G4 computer. EEG data are sampled at 500 Hz, 0.1 – 100 Hz analog filtered, referenced to the vertex. The Geodesic Sensor Net is a lightweight elastic thread structure containing Ag/AgCl electrodes housed in a synthetic sponge on a pedestal. The sponges are soaked in a KCl solution to render them conductive. Stimulus-locked EEG data are segmented off-line into 1000 ms epochs spanning 200 ms pre-stimulus to 800 ms post-stimulus around the critical stimulus events. For example in our task the events were: (1) neutral target, (2) neutral non-target, (3) traumatic stress target, (4) traumatic stress non-target; (5) drug target, (6) drug non-target. Frequency of targets for each emotional category was 20%. Data were digitally screened for artifact (eye blinks, movement, etc.) and bad trials were removed using built-in artifact rejection tools. The remaining data were sorted by condition and averaged to create the ERPs. Averaged ERP data were digitally filtered at 30 Hz lowpass to remove residual high-frequency noise before averaging. After averaging the baseline was corrected over a 200 ms baseline period relative to segment start, and data re-referenced into an average reference frame. The subject ERPs were averaged together to produce the mean grandaverage across subjects.
Pictorial stimuli
The emotional pictorial material were taken from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS , Lang et al., 2001). Cocaine images were selected and validated by the first author during his post-doctoral fellowship at Rice University (Houston, TX). In that prior study (Potts, Martin, Stotts, George, & Sokhadze, unpublished report), 25 cocaine abusing patients rated 115 cocaine-related images on 5-point scale (5 being high) as to how evocative each drug image was. The mean rating for the entire set was 2.66, SD=0.48. There were selected 30 images that had the highest rating (all 30 with mean rating above 3.0) for use in this study. Valence, arousal, and dominance rates were matched within each set of images in neutral and traumatic stress categories using ratings from the IAPS database (Lang et al., 2001). The experiment used pictures from three categories: neutral (household items, animals, nature), traumatic stress (violence, accidents, victims of attack, etc.), and drugs (cocaine and drug paraphernalia). Subjects were instructed to respond to stimulus items from one of the categories, ignoring the others within each block (e.g., targets are household items in a “neutral” block). The order of blocks (with 240 trials per block) was counter-balanced. In the task a stimulus was presented on a screen for 200 ms, whereas recording of EEG data occurred for 1000 ms. Inter-trial interval varied in 1500~2000 ms range to avoid anticipation effects. Each of three blocks of trials was followed by a short break. The task took approximately 30 minutes to complete.
Dependent variables
Behavioral variables were mean reaction time (RT) and response accuracy (in percent) to target stimuli, while electrophysiological variables were adaptive mean amplitude and latency of the frontal P3a, and the centro-parietal P3b. Statistical analyses were performed on the subject-averaged data with the subject averages being the observations. The primary analysis model was the repeated measures ANOVA, with physiological dependent variables being those described above. Therefore, each ERP component’s amplitude and latency were analyzed for pre-selected regions-of-interest (ROI) and time window. Time window was in 300-590 ms range for both P300 measures. The ROI for the frontal P3a included AFz, AF3, AF4, Fz, F1, F2, F3, F4, and four neighboring EEG sites (EGI channels 10,19, 5,12). Frontal EEG channels, AF3,F1,F3, EGI-19, and EGI-12 were used as the left frontal ROI, while channels AF4, F2, F4, EGI-5, and EGI-10 for the right frontal ROI. Analysis was performed as well for the midline frontal EEG sites (AFz, Fz). ROI for the centro-parietal P3b included Cz, CPz, Pz, CP1, CP2, CP3, CP4, and four neighboring EGI channels, and were calculated separately for left, right, and midline ROIs. Figure 1 illustrates the layout of Electrical Geodesics Sensor Net and ROIs.
Initially all dependent variables were analyzed using one-way ANOVA to find group differences (CNT vs. SUD, CNT vs. DUAL, SUD vs. DUAL, CNT vs. SUD+DUAL). Then data for selected dependent ERP variable were analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVA with following factors (all within-participants): Stimulus type × (target, non-target) × Cue Category (neutral, drug, trauma) × Hemisphere (left vs. right). Between subject factors in the tasks were Group (DUAL, SUD, CNT) and the following variations of grouping (CNT vs. DUAL; CNT vs. SUD, DUAL vs. SUD). Post-hoc analysis was conducted using Tukey test for groups with unequal sample size. A-priori hypotheses were tested with two-tailed Student’s t-tests for groups with unequal variance. In all ANOVAs, Greenhouse-Geisser (GG) corrected p-values were employed where appropriate. SPSS (v.14) and Sigma Stat 3.1 packages were used for statistical analysis. Topographic maps were created using spherical spine interpolation available in the EGI Net Station work-tools (v. 4.01).
RESULTS
Behavioral responses
Reaction time (RT) was globally slower in both SUD and DUAL groups compared to controls (CNT), however one-way ANOVA showed significance of RT differences between controls and addicts (both SUD and DUAL groups, SUD+DUAL) only for trauma targets (529.6 ± 55.9 ms CNT vs. 642.6 ± 121.9 all addicts, F(1,33)=6.25, p=0.018). These differences were very well expressed when CNT group was compared with DUAL group on targets of neutral and trauma (stress) categories. Stressful targets had main effect on RT across all subjects (517 ms neutral vs. 581 ms traumatic target, F(2,27)=15.18, p=0.001). There was also a trend to between-group difference on trauma targets (CNT vs. DUAL, F=(2,27)=4.63, p=0.046), and a marginally significant Category (neutral, trauma) × Group (CNT,DUAL) interaction (F=(4,36)=4.66, p=0.046), with RT to neutral targets being similar, while RT to trauma cues being slower in DUAL group. Target Category (neutral, trauma, drug) had main effect (shortest RT to neutral, longest to trauma, F(2,36)=4.89, p=0.016) showing that this manipulation of stimulus emotional category was affecting RT in all subjects. There were not any significant differences in RT between SUD and DUAL groups.
Accuracy
Comparison across all 3 groups did not yield any differences in error rate. However, when controls and addicts were compared separately, Cue Category (neutral, trauma, drug) × Group (CNT, SUD) interaction tendency was found, F(2,27)=3.98, p=0.043, which can be described as a tendency to lower error rate 5.89% (SUD) vs. 9.25% (CNT) on drug targets and a higher rate of errors on neutral targets (11.5% vs. 6.6%) in addicts. Comparisons of CNT and DUAL groups on the same accuracy measure also showed a trend toward Category × Group interaction (F(2,18)=3.86, p=0.049), with DUAL patients compared to controls committing more errors to trauma targets, but not to drug or neutral targets.
Event-Related Potentials
Data from one subject from DUAL and 2 subjects from SUD-only group were not included in the ERP analysis due to an excessive number of artifacts caused by movement, eye blinks etc. Therefore we report data on 9 controls (CNT group), 12 subjects with SUD without PTSD (SUD group), and 10 subjects with SUD-PTSD comorbidity (DUAL group). For certain controls vs. addiction group comparisons we included for analysis as well a combined addiction group (SUD+DUAL group).
Frontal P300 (P3a)
Amplitude of P3a
Cue Category (neutral, trauma, drug) had a main effect on P3a amplitude (F(2,28)=15.6, p=0.006) , with the highest amplitude of P3a component in trauma, while the lowest in drug cues. Stimulus (target, non-target) type also had a main effect (F(1,28)=7.33, p=0.011), with amplitude being higher to targets than to non-targets. Comparison of controls (N=9) with all addicts (both SUD and DUAL groups, N=21) showed a significant cue Category (neutral, trauma, drug) × Hemisphere (left, right) × Group interaction (F(2,27)=9.42, p=0.001), where addicts showed larger P3a to drug cues, but not to neutral cues, and manifested less hemispheric differences. Figures Figures22 and and33 illustrates higher amplitude of P3a to non-target drug-related cues in cocaine addicts. Effect of enhanced P3a was better expressed at the left rather then at the right frontal site. The same effect was observed when controls (CNT, N=9) were compared with addicts without PTSD (SUD, N=12): F(2,18)=4.12, p=0.03.
Comparisons of control and dual diagnosis groups showed a cue Category (neutral, trauma, drug) × Stimulus(target, non-target) × Group (CNT, DUAL) interaction effect (F(2,38)=4.52, p=0.038, GG corrected df=1.19), and a well manifested Category × Hemisphere × Group effect (F(2,38)=8.14, p=0.005). The effect can be described as a larger P3a to trauma targets than non-targets at the right frontal sites, and lower amplitude to neutral and drug non-targets than targets. Figure 4 shows this cue Category × Group interaction for target cues in control and dual subjects.
Latency of P3a
One-way ANOVA showed significant differences between three groups (CNT, SUD, DUAL) in the latency of P3a to neutral targets (F(2,29)=4.32, p=0.022), traumatic targets (F(2,29)=3.71, p=0.036) and non-targets (F(2,29)=7.65, p=0.002), drug targets (F(2,29)=4.55, p=0.019), and drug non-targets (at the right side only, F(2,29)=4.74, p=0.016). Dual patients showed a longer P3a latency to neutral targets and non-targets, whereas both SUD and DUAL groups had longer latencies to drug targets and non-targets than controls. Most interesting differences were revealed during comparison of addiction-only vs. dual patient groups. Stimulus type (target, non-target) had main effect (F(1,20)=5.52, p=0.03), but cue Category (neutral, trauma, cue) had no main effect on latency in these groups. Stimulus × Category × Group (SUD, DUAL) yielded significant interaction (F(2,38)=5.56, p=0.014). In particular, P3a latency was globally delayed both to target and non-target cues in DUAL patients compared to SUD patients, and was longer to non-target trauma, and to target trauma cues (Figure 5).
Centro-parietal P300 (P3b)
Amplitude of P3b
Both cue Category (F(2,28)=56.01, p=0.006) and Stimulus type (target, non-target) (F(1,29)=7.32, p=0.011) exerted main effect on the amplitude of P3b. Comparison of P3b between controls and addicts revealed Stimulus (target, non-target) × Hemisphere (left, right) × Group (CNT, all SUD) interaction, F(2,58)=4.21, p=0.03., Patient group had a lower P3b to neutral, but not to drug cues, and less differentiated hemispheric differences compared to controls. The P3b amplitude in addicts was higher in response to drug category cues at the left hemisphere. A Stimulus × Hemisphere × Group interaction was found as well when CNT and DUAL groups were compared (F(2,38)=3.86, p=0.031; GG corrected, df=1.59, p=0.042).
Latency of P3b
This measure showed a Hemisphere × Group interaction (F(1,28)=4.84, p=0.036 CNT vs. all SUD). This lower left-right hemispheric differences were better visible when CNT and SUD-only group where compared (F(1,28)=5.40, p=0.028). The same effect was marginally close, but not reaching significance level when CNT and DUAL groups were compared.
DISCUSSION
Our experiment tested the hypothesis that the stimulus evaluation cortical circuits have been conditioned to drug-cues in addiction group (drug cue reactivity), and conditioned to both drug- and stress-related cues in group of patients with cocaine addiction and PTSD comorbidity (drug- and stress-cue reactivity). The frontal P3a and centro-parietal P3b components were predicted to be larger to targets than non-targets in each category of images in all groups of subjects (CNT, SUD, DUAL), but P3a and P3b were predicted to be larger to drug-related (both targets and non-targets) cues in SUD-only group compared to controls, whereas larger to both drug- and stress-related categories in dually diagnosed subjects compared to controls and cocaine addicts without PTSD. Specifically such predictions assumed presence of a main effect for stimulus type (target, non-target), larger P300 to targets, but no Stimulus × Group interaction. At the same type our hypothesis predicted a main effect of Category (neutral, stress, drug), and a Category × Group interactions, namely larger ERPs to drug images in both groups of cocaine addicts (SUD, DUAL), and larger ERPs of interest to traumatic stress images in the DUAL group compared both to control and SUD groups.
Our predictions were partially confirmed by the obtained results. Our data showed the predicted larger P3a and P3b components to target stimuli (main effect for Stimulus), regardless of stimulus Category (neutral, stress, drug), in both the addicts and the controls, even though reactivity to non-target trauma and drug cues was globally higher in addiction groups compared to controls. Several higher order interactions (Stimulus × Category × Group; Category × Hemisphere × Group) were obtained for the amplitude and latency of P3a when addict groups were compared with the control group. The DUAL patients showed the predicted enhancement of P3a to traumatic stress cues (differentially to targets and non-targets) that reached significance, thus supporting the enhanced responsiveness and orienting to traumatic stress stimuli in the dually diagnosed patients. The group of addicted patients without PTSD showed the predicted larger frontal P3a to drug cue category, with P3a being larger at the left hemisphere, which is known to be involved in the processing of approach (appetitive) motivation tendencies (Davidson, 2002). It is worth noting that the centro-parietal P3b showed in our study similar but less pronounced Category × Group effects than the frontal P3a, suggesting that the P3a may be a more sensitive index of cue drug- and stress-related stimuli in cocaine addicts with comorbid PTSD.
While studies with active cocaine users have indicated a strong physical reaction to drug-related stimuli (Carter and Tiffany, 1999, Childress et al., 1993; Grant et al., 1996, London et al., 1999), research examining an attentional bias for cocaine-related stimuli has been limited (Franken et al., 2000). Our study extended the scope by using both drug- and stress-related cues in dually diagnosed patient group. Obtained data showed reduced reactivity to emotionally neutral and stressful images in cocaine addicts without PTSD. It has been shown that the experience of the emotions by psychostimulant substance abusers is distorted as a result of the dysregulation of the cerebral mechanisms involved in the motivational and emotional processes (Goldstein & Volkow, 2002; Volkow et al., 2004). The results are in accord with the reports from other studies that individuals with cocaine addiction produce low activation to natural affective stimuli, but present high activation in these brain structures in response to drug-related items (Garavan et al., 1999, 2000; Grant et al., 1996; Hester, Dixon, & Garavan, 2006).
It has been proposed that a sensitization of the motivational circuits toward stimuli associated with drugs could be associated with the motivational response of craving (Bonson et al., 2002; Robinson & Berridge, 1993), which could also provoke an inhibition of the emotional response to other natural reinforcement not related to drug use. One of the core features of addictive behavior is the preoccupation of drug dependent persons with drugs and drug paraphernalia that can be conceptualized according to Franken (2003) as an attentional bias. In cocaine addiction, items related to cocaine and drug paraphernalia are repeatedly selected by attention for conscious processing, and drug-related representations are disproportionately tagged as relevant.
Attentional bias toward processing of salient stimuli is hypothesized to be an implicit cognitive process which is poorly controlled. Such automatic processing is similar to the orienting reflex to novel signal. The automatic nature of addictive behaviors was outlined as well by other studies (Hester, Dixon, & Garavan, 2006; Lubman et al., 2000). Drug abuse-related after-effects in the medial prefrontal cortex (PFC) could be accompanied by impairments in emotional regulation, and specifically in inhibition of all motivations and emotions other than craving (London et al., 2000; Shalev, Grimm, & Shaham, 2002). Diminished PFC control of the fronto-striatal circuits allows more habitual responses mediated by the posterior and subcortical (e.g., basal ganglia, striatum) structures to take over regulation of behavior.
There is a converging evidence that implicit automatic processes are also involved in the fear processing (Mogg & Bradley, 1998). Neuroimaging studies showed that medial prefrontal cortical areas modulate fear responding through inhibitory connections with the amygdala (Davidson, 2002; Devinsky et al., 1995). It was hypothesized that dysfunction of the interaction of prefrontal and limbic structures plays a role in failure of extinction to fear in PTSD (Bremner et al., 1996, 1999, 2004). PTSD is often conceptualized in terms of conditioned fear with enhanced emotional memory acquisition mediated by a hyper-responsive amygdala and delayed extinction due to failure of inhibitory control of the medial PFC and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) over the amygdala (Charney et al., 1993; Gilboa et al., 2004; Grillon et al., 1998; Li & Sinha, 2008; Rauch et al., 1996). These PFC deficits may further enhance the effects of the amygdala hyperactivation, thereby increasing the frequency and intensity of PTSD symptoms (Bremner et al., 1999). Negative emotions typical for PTSD and decreased stress coping capacity may augment craving and promote drug-seeking and relapse behaviors (Goeders, 2003; Koob, 1999). In dually diagnosed individuals reactivity to both traumatic and drug cues may represent a combined conditioned and unconditioned response which increases vulnerability for further progression of drug use.
Drug addiction leads to frontal top-down control deficits. Deficient inhibitory control results in an inability to override strong habitual drug-seeking behaviors, thus allowing external salient cues (drug-related cues, and both drug- and stress-related in a case of comorbid PTSD), and pathological craving (and fear in PTSD) drive behavior. Individuals genetically predisposed to behavioral disinhibition are more vulnerable to impulsive drug abuse (Bauer, 1997). Reduced prefrontal inhibitory control results as well in a diminished capacity to override stress responses and generally poor stress coping skills (Koob & Le Moal, 2001; Li & Sinha, 2008; Sinha et al., 1999). Therefore, addictive behavior leads to functional abnormalities resulting in an imbalance in reward values due to hyper-sensitization to drug-stimuli and drug-associated motivation at the expense of a natural reinforcement. PTSD is further contributing to the severity of drug dependence through enhanced reactivity to traumatic stress-related external stimuli and negative emotional states in response to external internal cues (e.g., flashbacks, stress-related memories and ruminations, etc.).
Active cocaine use and cocaine withdrawal-related alterations in neural structures involved in stress response are well known (Koob et al., 2004), and these neuroadaptave changes in stress circuits, according to Li and Sinha (2008), may contribute to the increased salience of drug and drug-related stimuli in a variety of challenge or “stress” contexts (Robinson & Berridge, 2000; Sinha, 1999). Furthermore, they have also proposed that addiction-related alterations in cortico-striatal–limbic circuits may contribute to reduced coping ability, poor behavioral flexibility and deficient problem solving capacity during increasing levels of stress or emotional challenges in psychoactive stimulant users (Li & Sinha, 2008; Sinha et al., 2006).
This project studies specific components of event-related brain potentials and behavioral (reaction time and accuracy) measures to investigate reactivity to drug- and stress-related cues in individuals with cocaine use disorder with comorbid PTSD. It shows that cognitive task employing emotionally challenging cues could be used as a potentially useful diagnostic tool to assess cognitive and emotional functioning in cocaine abuse and PTSD. These ERP and behavioral parameters probably could be used as useful measures that can be employed to assess clinical and research outcomes in both pharmacologic and behavioral and neurofeedback interventions. These psychiatric and ERP-based cognitive functioning assessments were important part of our outpatient subjects’ clinical evaluations at the intake stage since most of cocaine addicts expressed willingness to enroll in an integrated behavioral treatment trial based on neurofeedback and motivational interviewing. These results contribute to a better understanding of the neurobiologic interaction between these mental disorders, and also offer a basis for a model explaining the high prevalence of this particular form of dual diagnosis by using cognitive neuroscience methods and theories.
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by the ISNR Research Committee grant and the NIDA R03DA021821 grant to Tato Sokhadze.
REFERENCES
- American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV) 4th ed. Washington, D.C.: 1994.
- Attias J, Bleich A, Furman V, Zinger Y. Event-related potentials in post-traumatic stress disorder of combat origin. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;40:373–381. [PubMed]
- Bauer LO. Frontal P300 decrement, childhood conduct disorder, family history, and predisposition of relapse among abstinent cocaine abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1997;44:1–10. [PubMed]
- Bauer LO, Kranzler HR. Electroencephalographic activity and mood in cocaine-dependent outpatients: effects of cocaine cue exposure. Biological Psychiatry. 1994;36:189–197. [PubMed]
- Biggins CA, MacKay S, Clark W, Fein G. Event-related potential evidence for frontal cortex effects of chronic cocaine dependence. Biological Psychiatry. 1997;42:472–485. [PubMed]
- Blanchard EB. Elevated basal levels of cardiovascular responses in Vietnam veterans with PTSD: a health problem in the making? J. Anxiety Disord. 1990;4:233–237.
- Blanchard EB, Hickling EJ, Buckley TC, Taylor AE, Vollmer A, Loos WR. The psychophysiology of motor vehicle accident related posttraumatic stress disorder: Replication and extension. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1996;64:742–751. [PubMed]
- Bleich A, Attias J, Furnam V. Effects of repeated visual traumatic stimuli on the event-related P3 brain potential in post-traumatic stress disorder. Int J Neuroscience. 1996;85:45–55. [PubMed]
- Blomhoff S, Reinvang I, Malt UF. Event-related potentials to stimuli with emotional impact in posttraumatic stress patients. Biological Psychiatry. 1998;44:1045–1053. [PubMed]
- Bonson KR, Grant SJ, Contoreggi CS, Links JM, Metcalfe J, et al. Neural systems and cue- induced cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26(3):376–386. [PubMed]
- Bremner JD, Southwick SM, Darnell A, Charney DS. Chronic PTSD in Vietnam combat veterans: course of illness and substance abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1996;153:369–375. [PubMed]
- Bremner JD, Staib LH, Kaloupek D, Southwick SM, Soufer R, Charney DS. Neural correlates of exposure to traumatic pictures and sound in Vietnam combat veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder A positron emission tomography study. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:806–816. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Bremner JD, Vermetten E, Vythilingam M, Afzal N, Schmahl C, Elzinga B, Charney DS. Neural correlates of the classic color and emotional stroop in women with abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;55:612–620. [PubMed]
- Brown PJ, Wolfe J. Substance abuse and post-traumatic stress disorder comorbidity. Drug Alcohol Dependence. 1994;35:51–59. [PubMed]
- Brown PJ, Recupero PR, Stout R. PTSD-substance abuse comorbidity and treatment utilization. Addictive Behavior. 1995;20:251–254. [PubMed]
- Carter BL, Tiffany ST. Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction. 1999;94:327–340. [PubMed]
- Casada JH, Amdur R, Larsen R, Liberzon I. Psychophysiologica responsivity in posttraumatic stress disorder: generalized hyperresponsiveness versus trauma specificity. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44:1037–1044. [PubMed]
- Charles G, Hansenne M, Ansseau M, Pitchot W, Machowski R, Schittecatte M, Wilmotte J. P300 in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychobiology. 1995;32:72–74. [PubMed]
- Charney DS, Deutch AY, Krystal JH, Southwick SM, Davis M. Psychobiologic mechanisms of posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:295–305. [PubMed]
- Chilcoat HD, Breslau N. Posttraumatic stress disorder and drug disorders: testing casual pathways. Archives General Psychiatry. 1998;55:913–917. [PubMed]
- Childress AR, Mozley D, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, et al. Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:11–18. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Coffey SF, Saladin ME, Drobes DJ, Brady KT, Dansky BS, Kilpatrick DG. Trauma and substance cue reactivity in individuals with comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder and cocaine or alcohol dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2002;65:115–127. [PubMed]
- Cox WM, Fadardi JS, Pothos EM. The addiction-stroop test: theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:443–476. [PubMed]
- Davidson RJ. Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;51:68–80. [PubMed]
- Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA. Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain. 1995;118(1):279–306. [PubMed]
- Derogatis LR, Lipman RS, Rickels K, Uhlenhuth EH, Covi L. The Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL): a self-report symptom inventory. Behav Sci. 1974;19:1–15. [PubMed]
- Donchin E, Coles MGH. Is the P300 a manifestation of context updating? Behav. Brain Science. 1988;11:357–374.
- Drake ME, Pakalnis A, Phillips B, Pamadan H, Hietter SA. Auditory evoked potentials in anxiety disorder. Clin. Electroencephalogr. 1991;22:97–101. [PubMed]
- Drummond DC, Tiffany ST, Glautier S, Remington B. Addictive Behaviour: Cue Exposure Theory and Practice. Wiley; Chichester: 1995.
- Evans K, Sullivan JM. Dual Diagnosis. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2001.
- Fein G, Bigging C, MacKay S. Cocaine abusers have reduced auditory P50 amplitude and suppression compared to both normal controls and alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry. 1996;39:955–965. [PubMed]
- Felmingham KL, Bryant RA, Kendall C, Gordon E. Event-related potential dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder: the role of numbing. Psychiatry research. 2002;109:171–179. [PubMed]
- First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR axis I disorders – patient edition (SCID – I/P) New York State Psychiatric Institute; New York: 2001.
- Foa EB, Steketee G, Rothbaum BO. Behavioral/cognitive conceptualisations of post-traumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy. 1989;20:155–176.
- Foa EB, Cashman L, Jaycox L, Perry K. The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder The Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale. Psychological Assessment. 1997;9:445–451.
- Franken IH, de Haan HA, van der Meer CW, Haffmans PM, Hendriks VM. Cue reactivity and effects of cue exposure in abstinent posttreatment drug users. Journal Substance Abuse Treatment. 1999;16:81–85. [PubMed]
- Franken IHA, Kroon LY, Hendriks VM. Influence of individual differences in craving and obsessive cocaine thoughts on attentional processes in cocaine abuse patients. Addictive Behaviours. 2000;25(1):99–102. [PubMed]
- Franken IHA. Drug craving and addiction: integrating psychological and psychopharmacological approaches. Progress Neuro-Pharmacology Biological Psychiatry. 2003;27:563–579. [PubMed]
- Friedman D, Simpson GV, Hamberger M. Age-related changes in scalp topography to novel and target stimuli. Psychophysiology. 1993;30:383–396. [PubMed]
- Friedman D, Squires-Wheeler E. Event related potentials as indicators of risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1994;20:63–74. [PubMed]
- Fukunishi I. Alexithymia in substance abuse: relationship to depression. Psychol. Rep. 1996;78:641–642. [PubMed]
- Gaeta H, Friedman D, Hunt G. Stimulus characteristics and task category dissociate the anterior and posteriror aspects of the novelty P3. Psychophysiology. 2003;40:198–208. [PubMed]
- Garavan H, Pankiewicz J, Bloom A, Cho J-K, Sperry L, et al. Cue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1789–1798. [PubMed]
- Garavan H, Ross TJ, Stein EA. Right hemisphere dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. Proceedings National Academy Science USA. 1999;96:8301–8306. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Gilboa A, Shalev AY, Laor L, Lester H, Louzoun Y, Chisin R, Bonne O. Functional connectivity of the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:263–272. [PubMed]
- Goeders NE. The impact of stress on addiction. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;13(6):435–441. [PubMed]
- Goldstein R, Volkow ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2002;159:1642–1652. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Grant S, London ED, Newlin DB, Villemagne VL, Liu X, Contoreggi C, Phillips RL, Kimes AS, Margolin A. Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sciences USA. 1996;93:12040–12045. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Grillon C, Morgan CA, Davis M, Southwick SM. Effects of experimental context and explicit threat cues on acoustic startle in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44:1027–1036. [PubMed]
- Handelsman L, Stein JA, Bernstein DP, Oppenheim SE, Rosenblum A, Magura S. A latent variable analysis of coexisting emotional deficits in substance abusers: alexithymia, hostility, and PTSD. Addict. Behav. 2000;25:423–428. [PubMed]
- Herning RI, Glover BJ, Guo X. Effects of cocaine on P3B in cocaine abusers. Neuropsychobiology. 1994;30:132–142. [PubMed]
- Hester R, Garavan H. Executive dysfunction in addiction: evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate, and cerebellar activity. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:11017–11022. [PubMed]
- Hester R, Dixon V, Garavan H. A consistent bias for drug-related material in active cocaine users across word and picture versions of the emotional Stroop task. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2006;81:251–257. [PubMed]
- Jacobsen LK, Southwick S, Kosten TR. Substance use disorders in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. American J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1184–1190. [PubMed]
- Johnson BA, Chen YR, Schmitz J, Bordnic P, Shafer A. Cue reactivivty in cocaine-dependent subjects: effects of cue type and cue modality. Addictive Behavior. 1998;23:7–15. [PubMed]
- Karl A, Malta LS, Maerker A. Meta-analytic reviw of event-related potential studies in post-traumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychology. 2006;71:123–147. [PubMed]
- Katayama J, Polich J. P300 from one-, two-, and three-stimulus auditory paradigms. International J Psychophysiology. 1996;23:33–40. [PubMed]
- Katayama J, Polich J. Stimulus context determines P3a and P3b. Psychophysiology. 1998;35:23–33. [PubMed]
- Kimble M, Kaloupek D, Kaufman M, Deldin P. Stimulus novelty differentially affects attentional allocation in PTSD. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;47:880–890. [PubMed]
- Kilts CD, Schweitzer JB, Quinn CK, Gross RE, Faber TL, Muhammad F, Ely TD, Hoffman JM, Drexler KP. Neural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction. Archive General Psychiatry. 2001;58:334–341. [PubMed]
- Kilts CD, Gross RE, Ely TD, Drexler KPG. The neural correlates of cue-induced craving in cocaine-dependent women. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:233–241. [PubMed]
- Knight RT. Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiology. 1984;59:9–20. [PubMed]
- Koob GF. Stress, corticotropin-releasing factor, and drug addiction. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1999;897:27–45. [PubMed]
- Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24:97–129. [PubMed]
- Koob GF, Ahmed SH, Boutrel B, Chen S, Kenny PJ, Markou A, O’Dell L, Parsons L, Sanna PP. Neurobiological mechanisms in the transition from drug use to drug dependence. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004;27:739–749. [PubMed]
- Kouri EM, Lukas SE, Mendelson JH. P300 assessment of opiate and cocaine users: effects of detoxification and buprenorphine treatment. Biological Psychiatry. 1996;60:617–628. [PubMed]
- Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. CRP, University Florida; 2001. (Tech. report A-5).
- Li CR, Sinha R. Inhibitory control and emotional stress regulation: Neuroimaging evidence for frontal–limbic dysfunction in psycho-stimulant addiction. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32:581–597. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- London ED, Ernst M, Grant S, Bonson K, Weinstein A. Orbitofrontal cortex and human drug abuse: functional imaging. Cerebral Cortex. 2000;10:334–342. [PubMed]
- Lubman D, Peters L, Mogg K, Bradley B, Deakin J. Attentional bias for drug cues in opiate dependence. Psychological Medicine. 2000;30:169–175. [PubMed]
- Lyvers M. “Loss of control”in alcoholism and drug addiction: a neuroscientific interpretation. Expertimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2000;8:225–249. [PubMed]
- McFarlane AC, Weber DL, Clark CR. Abnormal stimulus processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1993;34:311–320. [PubMed]
- McLellan AT, Luborsky L, Woody GE, O’Brien CP. An improved diagnostic evaluation instrument for substance abuse patients: The Addiction Severity Index. J Nervous Ment Dis. 1980;168:26–33. [PubMed]
- Metzger LJ, Orr SP, Lasko NB, Pitman RK. Auditory event-related potentials to tone stimuli in combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1997a;42:1006–1115. [PubMed]
- Metzger LJ, Orr SP, Lasko NB, McNally RJ, Pitman RK. Seeking the source of the emotional Stroop interference effects in PTSD: a study of P3s to traumatic words. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science. 1997b;32:43–51. [PubMed]
- Michalec EM, Rohsenow DJ, Monti PM, Varney SM, Martin RA, Dey AN, Myers M, Sirota AD. A Cocaine Negative Consequences Checklist: development and validation. J Substance Abuse. 1996;8:181–193. [PubMed]
- Mogg K, Bradley BP. A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behavioral Research Therapy. 1998;36:809–848. [PubMed]
- Naatanen R. The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive functioning. Behav Brain Sci. 1990;13:201–287.
- Najavits LM, Weiss RD, Shaw SR, Muenz LR. “Seeking safety”: Outcome of a new cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy for women with posttraumatic stress disorder and substance dependence. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1998;11:437–456. [PubMed]
- Noldy NE, Carlen PL. Event-related potential changes in cocaine withdrawal: evidence for long-term cognitive effects. Neuropsychobilogy. 1997;36:53–56. [PubMed]
- O’Brien CP, Charney DS, Lewis L, Cornish JW, Post R, et al. Priority actions to improve the care of persons with co-occurring substance abuse and other mental disorders: A call to action. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56:703–713. [PubMed]
- Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. [PubMed]
- Orr SP, Lasko NB, Metzger LJ, Berry NJ, Ahern CE, Pitman RK. Psychophysiologic assessment of women with PTSD resulting from childhood sexual abuse. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1998;66:906–913. [PubMed]
- Orr SP, Roth WT. Psychophysiological assessment: Clinical applications for PTSD. J Affect Disord. 2000;61:225–240. [PubMed]
- Ouimette PC, Ahrens C, Moos RH, Finney JW. Posttraumatic stress disorder in substance abuse patients: relationship to one-year posttreatment outcomes. Psychology Addict Behavior. 1997;11:34–47.
- Ouimette PC, Finney JW, Moos R. Two-year posttreatment functioning and coping of substance abuse patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychology Addictive Behaviors. 1999;13:105–114.
- Ouimette PC, Brown PJ. Trauma and Substance Abuse. APA; Washington, DC: 2003.
- Prins A, Kaloupek DG, Keane TM. Psychophysiological evidence for autonomic arousal and startle in traumatized adult populations. In: Friedman MJ, Charney DS, Deutch AY, editors. Neurobiological and Clinical Consequences of Stress: From Normal Adaptation to PTSD. Raven Press; New York: 1995. pp. 291–314.
- Polich J. P300, probability, and interstimulus interval. Psychophysiology. 1990;27:396–403. [PubMed]
- Polich J, Pollock VE, Bloom FE. Meta-analysis of P300 from males at risk for alcoholism. Psychol Bull. 1994;115:55–73. [PubMed]
- Polich J, Herbst KL. P300 as a clinical assay: Rationale, evaluation and findings. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2000;38:3–19. [PubMed]
- Potts GF, Patel SH, Azzam PN. Impact of instructed relevance on the visual ERP. International Journal Psychophysiology. 2004;52:197–209. [PubMed]
- Pritchard W. Psychophysiology of P300. Psychol. Bull. 1981;89:506–540. [PubMed]
- Pritchard W. Cognitive event-related potential correlates of schizophrenia. Psychol Bul. 1986;100:43–66. [PubMed]
- Pritchard W, Sokhadze E, Houlihan M. Effects of nicotine and smoking on event-related potentials: a review. Nicotine Tobacco Research. 2004;6:961–984. [PubMed]
- Rauch SL, van der Kolk BA, Fisler RE, Alpert NM, Orr SP, et al. A symptom provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron emission tomography and script-driven imagery. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1996;53:380–387. [PubMed]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews. 1993;18:247–291. [PubMed]
- Sahar T, Shalev AY, Porges SW. Vagal modulation of responses to mental challenge in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2001;49:637–643. [PubMed]
- Saladin ME, Drobes DJ, Coffey SF, Dansky BS, et al. PTSD symptom severity as a predictor of cue-elicited drug craving in victims of violent crime. Addictive Behaviours. 2003;28:1611–1629. [PubMed]
- Shalev AY, Orr SP, Pitman RK. Psychophysiologic assessment of traumatic imagery in Israeli civilian post-traumatic stress disorder patients. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1993;150:620–624. [PubMed]
- Shalev U, Grimm JW, Shaham Y. Neurobiology of relapse to heroin and cocaine seeking: a review. Pharmacology Reviews. 2002;54:1–42. [PubMed]
- Shiperd JC, Stafford J, Tanner LR. Predicting alcohol and drug abuse in Persian Gulf War veterans: what role do PTSD symptoms play? Addictive Behaviors. 2005;30:595–599. [PubMed]
- Sinha R, Catapano D, O’Malley S. Stress-induced craving and stress response in cocaine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology. 1999;142:343–351. [PubMed]
- Sinha R, Garcia P, Paliwal M, Kreek MJ, Rounsaville BJ. Stress-induced cocaine craving and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses are predictive of cocaine relapse outcomes. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:324–331. [PubMed]
- Sokhadze E, Stewart C, Hollifield M. Integrating cognitive neuroscience methods with neurofeedback therapy in treatment of substance use disorder comorbid with PTSD. Journal of Neurothrapy. 2007;11(2):13–44.
- Stanford MS, Vasterling JJ, Mathias CW, Constans JI, Houston RJ. Impact of threat relevance on P3 event-related potentials in combat- related post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research. 2001;102:125–137. [PubMed]
- Stewart SH, Pihl RO, Conrod PJ, Dongier M. Functional association among trauma, PTSD, and substance-related disorders. Addictive Behaviors. 1998;23:797–812. [PubMed]
- Stormark KM, Laberg JC, Nordby H, Hugdahl K. Alcoholics’ selective attention to alcohol stimuli: Automated processing? Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2000;61:18–23. [PubMed]
- Vasterling JJ, Brewin CR. Neuropsychology of PTSD. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2005.
- Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. The addicted human brain: insights from imaging studies. J. Clin. Invest. 2003;111:1444–1451. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. The addicted human brain viewed in the light of imaging studies: brain circuits and treatment strategies. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47:3–13. [PubMed]
- Weinstein AV. Visual ERPs evidence of enhanced processing of threatening information in anxious university students. Biol. Psychiatry. 1995;37:847–858. [PubMed]
- Weiss F, Ciccocioppo R, Parsons LH, Katner S, Liu X, Zorrilla EP, et al. Compulsive drug-seeking behavior and relapse. Neuroadaptation, stress, and conditioning factors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001;937:1–26. [PubMed]
- Weissman MM, Bothwell S. Assessment of social adjustment by patient self-report. Arch General Psychiatry. 1976;33:1111–1115. [PubMed]
- Wijers AA, Mulder G, Gunter TC, Smid HGOM. Brain potential analysis of selective attention. In: Neumann O, Sanders AF, editors. Handbook of perception and action. Vol 3: Attention. Academic Press; Tullamore, Ireland: 1996. pp. 333–387.