- Published: November 4, 2013
- DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078258
Abstract
The activation of dopamine receptors within the mesolimbic dopamine system is known to be involved in the initiation and maintenance of cocaine use. Expression of the D2 dopamine receptor subtype has been implicated as both a predisposing factor and consequence of chronic cocaine use. It is unclear whether there is a predictive relationship between D2 dopamine receptor function and cocaine sensitivity that would enable cocaine abuse. Therefore, we exploited individual differences in behavioral responses to D2 dopamine receptor stimulation to test its relationship with cocaine-mediated behaviors. Outbred, male Sprague-Dawley rats were initially characterized by their locomotor responsiveness to the D2 dopamine receptor agonist, quinpirole, in a within-session ascending dose-response regimen (0, 0.1, 0.3 & 1.0 mg/kg, sc). Rats were classified as high or low quinpirole responders (HD2 and LD2, respectively) by a median split of their quinpirole-induced locomotor activity. Rats were subsequently tested for differences in the psychostimulant effects of cocaine by measuring changes in cocaine-induced locomotor activity (5 and 15 mg/kg, ip). Rats were also tested for differences in the development of conditioned place preference to a low dose of cocaine (7.5 mg/kg, ip) that does not reliably produce a cocaine conditioned place preference. Finally, rats were tested for acquisition of cocaine self-administration and maintenance responding on fixed ratio 1 and 5 schedules of reinforcement, respectively. Results demonstrate that HD2 rats have enhanced sensitivity to the locomotor stimulating properties of cocaine, display greater cocaine conditioned place preference, and self-administer more cocaine compared to LD2 animals. These findings suggest that individual differences in D2 dopamine receptor sensitivity may be predictive of cocaine sensitivity and reward.
Figures
Citation:Merritt KE, Bachtell RK (2013) Initial D2 Dopamine Receptor Sensitivity Predicts Cocaine Sensitivity and Reward in Rats. PLoS ONE 8(11): e78258. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078258
Editor: Abraham A. Palmer, University of Chicago, United States of America
Received: May 28, 2013; Accepted: September 10, 2013; Published: November 4, 2013
Copyright: © 2013 Merritt, Bachtell. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding:This work was supported by R03 DA 029420; CU Innovative Seed Grant. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Understanding why some individuals develop substance abuse or patterns of compulsive drug use while others do not is one of the most poorly understood aspects in the development of drug addiction. Epidemiological studies report that nearly 17% of people who use cocaine will become cocaine dependent within 10 years of initial cocaine use [1]. This suggests that some individuals are vulnerable, while others are resistant to developing drug dependence despite having a history of drug use. While there are many factors that may contribute to drug dependence (e.g. drug availability, social pressures, etc.), the discrepancy between vulnerable and resistant individuals may also be explained through individual differences in the functioning of the neurobiological systems underlying the responsiveness to drugs of abuse [2]. Understanding these differences may provide insight into one of the most sought after questions in the development of substance dependence.
The mesolimbic dopamine (DA) system consists of dopamine cells in the ventral tegmental area that project to medium spiny neurons in the nucleus accumbens among other limbic regions [3]. Cocaine rapidly elevates extracellular DA in the terminal regions of mesolimbic pathway by blocking the DA transporter, which contributes to cocaine reinforcement [4]. Activation of the mesolimbic pathway is widely known to be involved in the initiation and maintenance of cocaine use and use of other drugs of abuse [5]. Alterations within mesolimbic DA circuitry have been demonstrated as both a consequence of repeated psychostimulant use and as a predisposing factor. For example, chronic cocaine use is associated with decreased D2 DA receptor levels in the ventral striatum of cocaine abusers [6], suggesting that decreased D2 DA receptor expression is a consequence of chronic cocaine administration. There has been a long-standing debate about whether the decrease in D2 DA receptor expression observed in cocaine abusers is a result of chronic cocaine use or whether this alteration represents a pre-existing conditioning that may predispose an individual to develop cocaine dependence.
Recent work in humans and animals suggests that reduced D2 DA receptor expression may in fact be a vulnerability factor. Thus, non-addicted individuals with lower levels of D2 DA receptor report greater drug “liking” for the psychostimulant, methylphenidate [7]. Mutant mice lacking the D2 DA receptor self-administer more cocaine compared to wild-type animals [8], while over-expressing D2 DA receptors in the ventral striatum decrease cocaine self-administration [9]. Together these studies suggest that pre-existing alterations in D2 DA receptor expression may predict the reinforcing effects of cocaine, although there are still uncertainties concerning the specific role of D2 DA receptors as a vulnerability factor.
There is emerging interest in the dissociation between D2 DA receptor expression and D2 DA receptor function and sensitivity. While binge-like cocaine administration in rats recapitulates decreased D2 DA receptor expression, as observed in human cocaine abusers, there are somewhat paradoxical increases G protein activation in response to D2 DA receptor stimulation [10]. Likewise, cocaine self-administration increases the expression of high affinity D2 DA receptors [10], [11]. These changes suggest that while the expression of D2 DA receptors may decrease, the sensitivity of D2 DA receptors may increase following repeated cocaine. This notion is reflected in several behavioral paradigms where chronic cocaine produces cross-sensitization to the psychostimulant effects of D2 DA receptor agonists [12], [13], [14], [15], and stimulation of D2 DA receptors produces robust reinstatement to cocaine seeking in rodent self-administration models [16], [17], [18], [19], [20], [21]. It is unknown whether the pre-existing differences in the sensitivity of D2 DA receptors relate to the behavioral effects of cocaine.
In the present studies, we utilized a rodent model to identify how individual differences in the behavioral sensitivity of D2 DA receptors relate to cocaine-induced behaviors. Administration of the D2 DA receptor agonist, quinpirole, produces a high degree of variability in locomotor responses in drug naïve animals. Thus, we exploited these individual differences in the rat’s initial locomotor response to quinpirole as a model to test D2 DA receptor sensitivity as a vulnerability factor for subsequent cocaine-mediated behaviors. Those animals displaying robust increases in quinpirole-induced activity were characterized as having high D2 DA receptor sensitivity (HD2), while those rats having more modest activation were characterized as having low D2 DA receptor sensitivity (LD2). Following this initial characterization, rats from each group were compared in cocaine-induced locomotion, cocaine-induced place preference, and cocaine self-administration.
Materials and Methods
Animals
Male Sprague–Dawley rats (Charles River, Portage, MI) weighing 275–325 g were individually housed upon arrival. Rats were given ad libitum food and water, except where indicated. All experiments were conducted during the light period of a (12:12) light/dark cycle.
Ethics Statement
These studies were carried out in accordance with the guidelines established by the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Colorado at Boulder.
Habituation to a Novel Environment
Locomotor activity was recorded in plexiglass chambers (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA) measuring 16×16×15 in with 16 pairs of photobeams spaced 1 in apart across both horizontal planes. All locomotor tests were performed in unlit activity chambers during the light phase of the (12:12) light/dark cycle. Animals were initially habituated to the novel locomotor testing chambers for 2 hrs prior to quinpirole-induced locomotor testing (see below).
Characterization of the Quinpirole-induced Locomotor Behavior
The initial locomotor response to the D2 DA receptor agonist, quinpirole was used to classify animals into groups prior to any further behavioral testing. Tests began at least 7 days after the animals arrived from the vendor and were conducted in darkened locomotor chambers during the light period of a (12:12) light/dark cycle. All animals were handled for approximately 5 min daily for 4 days prior to beginning these procedures to eliminate any potential interference. All animals were first habituated to the locomotor testing apparatus for 2 hrs the day prior to quinpirole testing (see above). Quinpirole-induced locomotion was assessed in a 5-hr within-session dose-response protocol as follows: 1-hr habituation followed by hourly ascending doses of the agonist (0, 0.1, 0.3 and 1.0 mg/kg, s.c.). A median split of total quinpirole-induced locomotor activity (calculated as Area Under the Curve, see below) was used to classify these rats as either high D2 responders (HD2) or low D2 responders (LD2). These procedures were conducted identically in several cohorts of animals (groups of rats arriving from the same vendor at identical age and weights) for each of the behavioral measures described (i.e. cocaine locomotion, place conditioning and self-administration). In each of the cohorts, the animal with the median score was tested, but eliminated from further data analyses. The distribution of scores within each cohort was qualitatively quite similar, but we did observe differences in the range and median scores for quinpirole-induced locomotor activity between cohorts of animals. Therefore, HD2 and LD2 classifications were made within each individual cohort.
Cocaine-induced Locomotor Behavior
In one cohort of animals (N = 39), locomotor responses were measured using a 3-hr within-session cocaine dose-response protocol. These assessments were performed in darkened locomotor chambers during the light period of a (12:12) light/dark cycle. Animals were tested 5–7 days following the initial characterization of their quinpirole sensitivity in the same activity chambers. On the test day, animals were habituated to the locomotor chamber for 1 hr and were then administered hourly ascending doses of cocaine (5 and 15 mg/kg, i.p.).
Cocaine Place Conditioning
In another cohort of animals (N = 37), place conditioning was measured in an unbiased 3-chamber apparatus using an unbiased 3-phase procedure. Testing began 7 days following the initial characterization of quinpirole sensitivity. The two conditioning chambers (15 cm×25 cm×35 cm) were distinct in wall patterns (gray vs. vertical white and black stripes) and floor textures (grid vs. hole). The center compartment (15 cm×10 cm) had white walls and a plexiglass floor. Chambers are equipped with infrared photocells to detect animal position and movement in the apparatus. From 1000–1500 hrs on the day before conditioning (pre-conditioning), rats were allowed access to all three compartments for 20 min to test for initial bias. One animal was excluded from the experiment because it displayed an initial bias of 92% time in one compartment. Rats received three 30-min saline conditioning sessions and three 30-min cocaine (7.5 mg/kg, i.p.) conditioning sessions. Saline conditioning occurred between 0800–1100 hrs, while cocaine conditioning occurred between 1500–1700 hrs. The 7.5 mg/kg cocaine dose was chosen because preliminary studies in our lab demonstrate that it does not reliably produce a place preference in all rats. Therefore, this cocaine dose was ideal to identify potential differences in the development of a place preference between the two groups. The final test session (post-conditioning) was conducted between 1000 hrs and 1500 hrs and rats were again allowed free access to the three compartments and preference was determined as time spent in the drug compartment minus time spent in the saline compartment (conditioned place preference (CPP) score).
Sucrose and Cocaine Self-administration
Another cohort of animals (N = 29) was tested for operant responding for sucrose pellets following the initial characterization of quinpirole sensitivity. Self-administration procedures were performed in operant conditioning chambers (Med-Associates, St Albans, VT) equipped with two response levers. Seven days following the initial quinpirole testing, these rats were food-restricted to prevent weight gain, and trained to lever-press for sucrose pellets on a fixed ratio 1 (FR1) reinforcement schedule until acquisition criteria had been achieved (50 sucrose pellets). The latency to reach this criterion was used as the dependent variable in these experiments. All rats reached criterion after approximately 8 days of training and were fed ad libitum thereafter.
Following the sucrose self-administration and at least one day of ad libitum feeding, animals were implanted with jugular catheters under halothane anesthesia (1–2.5%), as described elsewhere [22]. After 5–7 days of recovery from surgery, animals self-administered cocaine (0.5 mg/kg/100 µl, iv) under a FR1, timeout 20 s reinforcement schedule during 6 daily 2-h sessions. Animals were then transferred to a FR5, timeout 20 s schedule of reinforcement for an additional 5 daily 2-h sessions. Cocaine infusions were delivered over 5 s concurrent with the termination of the house light and illumination of a cue light above the drug-paired lever.
Drugs
Quinpirole [(-)-Quinpirole hydrochloride] and cocaine hydrochloride were purchased from Sigma (St. Louis, MO). All drugs were dissolved in sterile-filtered physiological (0.9%) saline.
Data Analysis
Cocaine-induced locomotor data (beam breaks) were analyzed by 2-factor mixed design ANOVA with quinpirole group (HD2 and LD2) and cocaine dose (5 & 15 mg/kg) as factors. Linear regressions were also performed on the locomotor data to identify the explanatory power of the quinpirole sensitivity in cocaine locomotion. Place conditioning data (CPP score = drug-paired minus saline-paired) was analyzed using a 2-factor mixed design ANOVA with quinpirole group (HD2 and LD2) and conditioning (Pre-conditioning and Post-conditioning) as factors. Cocaine self-administration data (cocaine infusions) were analyzed by both a 2-factor mixed design ANOVA with quinpirole group (HD2 and LD2) and days as factors, or an independent t-test between the quinpirole groups (HD2 and LD2) when cocaine infusions were collapsed across days. In all cases, significant main and interactive effects were followed by simple effects analyses and post hoc tests (Bonferroni’s test of significance). Statistical significance was preset at p<0.05.
Results
Characterization of High and Low Quinpirole Sensitivity Groups
There is a high degree of variation in responding across each quinpirole dose during the within-session dose response locomotor activity testing (Figure S1). Generally, the lowest dose of quinpirole (0.1 mg/kg, sc) suppresses locomotion compared to vehicle responding, while the higher doses (0.3 and 1.0 mg/kg, sc) activate locomotion. This is a prototypical quinpirole dose response, where low doses of quinpirole presumably stimulate D2 autoreceptors on dopamine terminals and higher quinpirole doses saturate D2 autoreceptors and stimulate postsynaptic D2 receptors [23], [24], [25]. In an attempt to capture the behavioral complexity of pre- and postsynaptic D2 receptor stimulation, we calculated the area under the curve (AUC) for each animal across all quinpirole doses (Figure S1). The quinpirole AUC score was then used to segregate each cohort into high quinpirole sensitivity (HD2) and low quinpirole sensitivity (LD2) groups based on a median split of the entire cohort. Figure 1A and 1B illustrate both the distribution of the quinpirole AUC scores and the group means following the median split into HD2 and LD2 groups. Figure 1C and 1D shows the distributions and group means of locomotion at each quinpirole dose. In developing the groups, the rat corresponding to the median score was eliminated from further analysis, but is shown on the graph to depict both the individual and mean range from the median score.
Download:
Figure 1. Distributions and averages of quinpirole-induced locomotor activity for LD2 and HD2 groups.
(A) Group distributions of the calculated quinpirole area under the curve (AUC) scores used to classify rats into the LD2 and HD2 groups. The dotted line represents the median score (M = 15460). (B) Group averages (± sem) of the quinpirole AUC score used to generate the LD2 and HD2 groups. The dotted line represents the median score (M = 15460). (C) Distribution of locomotor activity scores (beam breaks/hr) during the ascending within-session quinpirole dose response testing within the LD2 (gray circles) and HD2 (red circles) groups. (D) Group averages (± sem) of the quinpirole dose response curve for the LD2 and HD2 groups.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078258.g001
Given that the group assignments are primarily influenced by locomotor activation produced by quinpirole activation of postsynaptic D2 receptors, we also wanted to identify whether the groups differed in their responsiveness to the low, locomotor suppressing dose of quinpirole (0.1 mg/kg). To fully capture the magnitude of the suppressive effects of the low quinpirole dose, we calculated the suppressive effects of quinpirole as a percent of baseline (saline-induced activity; Figure S2). There were no differences in the quinpirole-induced locomotor suppression produced by 0.1 mg/kg quinpirole (t36 = 1.01, p = 0.3183), suggesting that the differential sensitivity to quinpirole between the HD2 and LD2 animals largely reflects the sensitivity of postsynaptic D2 DA receptors.
High Quinpirole Sensitivity Predicts Increased Cocaine-induced Locomotion
Utilizing the median split group assignments for quinpirole responding, we tested whether quinpirole sensitivity was related to the locomotor activating properties of cocaine. Figure 2 illustrates that HD2 animals had greater cocaine-induced locomotor activity following the 15 mg/kg cocaine dose, but not following the 5 mg/kg cocaine dose. A two-way mixed design ANOVA of these data reveal a significant interaction between cocaine dose and quinpirole group (F1,36 = 7.17, p = 0.0111), and main effects of cocaine (F1,36 = 88.43, p<0.0001) and group (F1,36 = 6.86, p = 0.0128). Figure 2 also displays the results of linear regressions performed at each cocaine dose across the entire population of animals. There was a significant relationship between quinpirole sensitivity and 15 mg/kg cocaine-induced locomotor activity (F1,36 = 8.62, p = 0.0058), but not 5 mg/kg cocaine-induced locomotor activity (F1,36 = 1.91, p = 0.1761). Thus, initial quinpirole sensitivity appears to predict cocaine-induced locomotion to a high, locomotor activating dose of cocaine.
Download:
Figure 2. HD2 animals display greater sensitivity to cocaine-induced locomotor activity.
(A) Rats were tested across two cocaine doses (5 and 15 mg/kg, ip) in a within-session procedure. HD2 animals displayed significantly greater cocaine-induced locomotor activity to 15 mg/kg cocaine, but not 5 mg/kg cocaine. *HD2 significant from LD2, p<0.05 (B and C) Analyses of the entire cohort were conducted to determine the relationship between quinpirole AUC scores and cocaine-induced locomotion. A non-significant positive relationship was identified for cocaine-induced activity at the low dose (B, 5 mg/kg cocaine) and a significant positive relationship was identified for cocaine-induced activity at the high dose (C, 15 mg/kg cocaine).
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078258.g002
Previous work demonstrates that novelty-induced locomotion is predictive of future cocaine responding [26], [27]. Therefore, we wanted to assess if there were differences between LD2 and HD2 groups in novelty-induced locomotor activity. There was no difference between the HD2 and LD2 groups in novelty-induced locomotion across the entire session (Figure 3A: t36 = 0.44, p = 0.6601) or within the first 30–60 minutes (Figure 3B), when differences in novelty responsiveness are typically most robust. To identify whether novelty-induced locomotor activity was predictive of D2 DA receptor sensitivity, we re-characterized our rats as having either low or high novelty-induced locomotor activity. Thus, we created low responding rats (LR) and high responding rats (HR) based on a median split of their initial locomotor responsiveness to the locomotor testing apparatus during the habituation phase of testing. We then determined whether these groups differed in quinpirole-induced locomotor activity. As shown in Figure 3, LR and HR rats did not differ significantly at any of the quinpirole doses (Group: F1,108<1, NS; Quinpirole: F3,108 = 69.61, p<0.0001; Interaction: (F3,108<1, NS), although the groups did significantly differ in cocaine-induced locomotion (Group: F1,36 = 10.49, p = 0.0026; Cocaine: F1,36 = 84.86, p<0.0001; Interaction: (F1,36 = 5.02, p = 0.0313). Together, these data suggest that while novelty-induced locomotion is predictive of cocaine responsiveness, the mechanisms associated with this relationship may be distinct from those associated with D2 DA receptor sensitivity.
Download:
Figure 3. Quinpirole sensitivity is not associated with novelty-induced locomotor activity.
Assessing novelty-induced locomotion during the habituation phase of testing revealed no significant differences between the LD2 and HD2 groups. (A) Distribution of novelty-induced locomotor activity scores over the 2-hr testing period. (B) Time course depicting novelty-induced locomotor activity between the LD2 and HD2 groups. Animals from this cohort were re-classified into a low responder group (LR) and high responder group (HR) based on their novelty-induced locomotor activity. (C) LR and HR rats did not predict differences in locomotor activity across the quinpirole dose response testing. (D) HR rats displayed significantly greater cocaine-induced locomotor activity across both cocaine doses. *HR significant from LR, p<0.05.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078258.g003
Since individual differences in the initial locomotor response to cocaine have also been shown to correspond with alterations in the development of cocaine sensitization, cocaine reward and cocaine self-administration, we re-characterized our rats as having either low or high cocaine-induced locomotor activity [28], [29], [30], [31]. This re-characterization was based on calculating the AUC for cocaine-induced locomotion across both cocaine doses during the within-session cocaine dose response testing. Rats having AUC values below the median were placed in the low cocaine responder (LCR) group while those having AUC values above the median were placed in the high cocaine responder (HCR) group. We then determined whether initial cocaine-induced locomotion was predictive of quinpirole-induced activity. HCR rats had greater overall quinpirole-induced activity compared to LCR rats using the quinpirole AUC score (t36 = 3.585, p<0.0010, data not shown). Analysis of the activity across the quinpirole dose response testing suggests that these differences were primary observed at the locomotor activating quinpirole doses (Figure 4). Thus, analysis of the quinpirole dose response between the groups revealed a significant main effects of group (F1,108 = 14.05, p = 0.0006), quinpirole dose (F3,108 = 85.93, p<0.0001) and the interaction (F3,108 = 7.64, p = 0.0001). We also assessed the relationship between the overall cocaine sensitivity and quinpirole sensitivity using the AUC scores for each drug where there was a significant correlation between the two activity scores (Figure 4). Together these findings suggest that there is significant overlap between the initial cocaine sensitivity and initial quinpirole sensitivity.
Download:
Figure 4. Initial cocaine sensitivity corresponds with differences in D2 DA receptor sensitivity.
The area under the curve (AUC) was calculated for each rat’s cocaine-induced locomotor activity across both 5 and 15 mg/kg doses. Using this calculated score for initial cocaine-induced locomotor activity, rats were re-classified into a low cocaine responder group (LCR) and a high cocaine responder group (HCR). (A) HCR rats displayed significantly greater quinpirole-induced locomotor activity at the 0.3 and 1.0 mg/kg doses. *HCR significant from LCR, p<0.05. (B) An analysis of the entire cohort was conducted to determine the relationship between quinpirole AUC scores and cocaine AUC scores. A significant positive relationship was identified between initial quinpirole sensitivity and initial cocaine sensitivity.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078258.g004
High Quinpirole Sensitivity Predicts Increased Cocaine Reward
In a separate cohort of animals, median split group assignments for quinpirole responding was created (data not shown) and place conditioning for cocaine (7.5 mg/kg) was tested. This dose was used in this test because it does not reliably produce robust place conditioning in all animals. Figure 5 illustrates both the saline- and cocaine-induced locomotion during the 30 min conditioning sessions. There was no significant group difference in saline-induced locomotion (F1,66 = 0.51, p = 0.4784). There was a significant decrease in saline-induced locomotion across each conditioning session (F2,66 = 10.91, p<0.0001) although there was no significant interaction between groups and sessions (F2,66 = 0.59, p = 0.5567). HD2 rats had significantly higher cocaine-induced locomotion during the conditioning sessions compared to LD2 rats (F1,66 = 4.29, p = 0.0462). There was no main effect of session (F2,66 = 0.77, p = 0.4595) and no significant interactive effects (F2,66 = 0.60, p = 0.5535), although qualitatively there appeared to be enhanced cocaine-induced locomotion during the first two conditioning sessions (Figure 5). Heightened cocaine-induced locomotion in HD2 animals during the conditioning sessions recapitulates our previous findings (Figure 2) and indicates that HD2 animals are more sensitive to the locomotor stimulating properties of cocaine and that may be predictive of cocaine reward. When the entire cohort was analyzed for the development of a conditioned place preference for cocaine, there was a significant increase in time spent in the cocaine-paired compartment post-conditioning (t36 = 2.27, p = 0.0295). When group was included in the analysis, there was a significant main effect of conditioning (F1,34 = 6.31, p = 0.0169), again suggesting that overall, animals developed a preference for the cocaine-paired compartment. There was no group effect (F1,34 = 3.27, p = 0.0793), but there was a significant interaction between conditioning and group (F2,34 = 4.36, p = 0.0443). Subsequent analyses revealed that HD2 animals displayed greater conditioned place preference to 7.5 mg/kg cocaine compared to LD2 animals on the post-conditioning test (t34 = 2.33, p = 0.0258), but did not differ on pre-conditioning test (t34 = 0.31, p = 0.7619). These findings suggest that initial quinpirole sensitivity is associated with heighted cocaine reward.
Download:
Figure 5. HD2 animals display greater sensitivity to the rewarding effects of cocaine.
(A) There were no group differences in the saline-induced locomotor activity during the conditioning trials. (B) There was a significant group difference in the cocaine-induced activity during the conditioning trials where HD2 animals displayed significantly greater cocaine-induced locomotor activity across all session. *HD2 significant from LD2, p<0.05. (C) Analyses of all animals in the cohort demonstrated a significant, modest cocaine-induced place preference following conditioning. † Post-conditioning significant from pre-conditioning, t36 = 2.27, p = 0.0295. (D) Group analyses demonstrated that only animals in the HD2 group developed a significant preference for the cocaine-paired compartment compared to animals in the LD2 group that did not develop any significant conditioning to the cocaine-paired compartment. *HD2 significant from LD2, p<0.05.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078258.g005
High Quinpirole Sensitivity Predicts Increased Cocaine Self-administration
In a separate cohort of animals, median split group assignments for quinpirole responding was created and self-administration of either sucrose or cocaine was tested. Figure 6 illustrates that there was no group difference in the acquisition of sucrose self-administration (F1,176 = 0.39, p = 0.5406) and both groups acquired equivalently (Sessions: F8,176 = 18.00, p<0.0001; Group×Session Interaction: F8,176 = 1.81, p = 0.0775), suggesting that these groups do not differ in reinforced learning of an operant response. These same animals were then implanted with a chronic indwelling catheter and allowed to self-administer cocaine. Animals initially acquired cocaine self-administration on an FR 1 schedule. There was a trend for HD2 to self-administer more cocaine than LD2 animals on an FR 1 schedule analyzed across all sessions (F1,95 = 3.31, p = 0.0846). When sessions were averaged across all FR 1 sessions, HD2 animals self-administered significantly more cocaine than LD2 animals (t19 = 2.63, p = 0.0164, data not shown). When the schedule was advanced to an FR 5 schedule of reinforcement HD2 animals self-administered more cocaine across sessions as revealed by a significant interaction (F4,76 = 3.465, p = 0.0118), although this effect was not observed when averaged across all FR 5 sessions (t19 = 1.51, p = 0.1484, data not shown). Thus, enhanced initial quinpirole sensitivity is associated with increased cocaine intake.
Download:
Figure 6. HD2 animals self-administer more cocaine than LD2 animals.
(A) There were no group differences in the acquisition of an operant response to acquire sucrose pellets. (B) There were significant group differences in the number of cocaine infusions delivered on both a fixed ratio 1 and fixed ratio 5 schedule of reinforcement. #significant trend between HD2 and LD2 groups, p = 0.08, *HD2 significant from LD2, p<0.05.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078258.g006
Cocaine Increases Quinpirole Sensitivity in both HD2 and LD2 Animals
It is well established that chronic cocaine treatments increase the sensitivity of D2 DA receptors [12], [13], [14], [15]. Therefore, we tested quinpirole sensitivity in all animals following the cocaine self-administration procedure to identify whether the pre-existing differences in D2 DA receptor sensitivity persisted following chronic cocaine administration. This was performed in all but 3 animals that were lost due to catheter failure. Figure 7 illustrates that cocaine self-administration enhances quinpirole-induced locomotion compared with responding in the same animals prior to cocaine self-administration. A two-way mixed ANOVA reveals that there was a main effect of cocaine exposure (F1,104 = 17.46, p<0.0001) and quinpirole dose (F2,104 = 66.73, p<0.0001). There was also a significant interaction (F2,104 = 10.61, p<0.0001). Similar results were obtained using the quinpirole AUC scores generated before and after cocaine exposure (t24 = 5.56, p<0.0001). We also analyzed the differences between HD2 and LD2 groups on quinpirole sensitivity before and after cocaine self-administration (Figure 7). Interestingly, pre-existing group differences remained despite cocaine-induced enhancements in D2 receptor sensitivity in both groups. Thus, analyses reveal a main effect of group (F3,98 = 24.21, p<0.0001), quinpirole dose (F2,98 = 117.50, p<0.0001) and the interaction (F6,98 = 16.03, p<0.0001). Similarly, results were also obtained using the quinpirole AUC scores generated before and after cocaine exposure. Analyses reveal a main effect of group (F1,23 = 46.05, p<0.0001) and cocaine exposure (F1,23 = 36.26, p<0.0001), but not the interaction (F1,23 = 3.45, p = 0.0760). These findings suggest that even though quinpirole sensitivity prior to cocaine self-administration predicts future cocaine responding, both populations develop quinpirole cross-sensitization following cocaine self-administration.
Download:
Figure 7. Cocaine self-administration enhances D2 DA receptor sensitivity in both LD2 and HD2 rats.
(A) Quinpirole AUC scores were enhanced across the entire cohort of animals tested following cocaine self-administration. *After cocaine significant from Before cocaine, p<0.05 (B) Likewise, this enhancement was observed across all quinpirole doses. *After cocaine significant from Before cocaine, p<0.05. (C and D) Cocaine-induced enhancements in D2 DA receptor sensitivity were apparent in both the LD2 and HD2 groups using both the quinpirole AUC scores and raw locomotor scores across the quinpirole dose response curve. *After cocaine significant from Before cocaine, p<0.05. Interestingly, the group differences persisted even after cocaine exposure. † HD2 significant from LD2, p<0.05.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078258.g007
Discussion
The findings reported here demonstrate that individual differences in the locomotor responsiveness to quinpirole are predictive of cocaine-induced behavioral regulation. This is the first demonstration that differences in the sensitivity of D2 DA receptors predict differential cocaine-induced locomotion, place preference and self-administration. The rats categorized as HD2, having high locomotor activation in response to quinpirole treatments, demonstrate increased cocaine-induced locomotor activity, increased cocaine reward, and self-administer cocaine in greater amounts compared to rats categorized as LD2 that have diminished locomotor activation in response to quinpirole. Importantly, categorizations of HD2 and LD2 did not parallel differences in the exploration of a novel environment, which has been show to be predictive of cocaine responding. Categorizing rats based on their initial cocaine sensitivity (HCR and LCR) did correspond with differences in quinpirole sensitivity suggesting that there may be common mechanisms underlying the individual differences between these two behavioral characteristics. It was determined that the categorization of HD2 and LD2 did not correspond with the quinpirole-induced suppression of locomotion that is presumably mediated by presynaptic D2 DA receptor stimulation [23], [24], [25]. Therefore, we suspect that the HD2 and LD2 group characterization in quinpirole locomotion likely reflects differences in the sensitivity of postsynaptic D2 DA receptors. However, quinpirole is also known to interact with some selectivity at D3 DA receptors [32]. In fact, it has been postulated that low doses of quinpirole induce increased oral behavior and yawning behavior in male rats through its interaction with D3 DA receptors [33], [34]. Thus, while we speculate that quinpirole-induced locomotion is reflective of postsynaptic D2 DA receptor stimulation, it is possible that D3 DA receptors may play a role in the behavioral responsiveness to quinpirole.
Alterations within the mesocorticolimbic DA circuitry have been long implicated as both a predisposing factor to psychostimulant use and a consequence of repeated psychostimulant use. The D2 DA receptor has received an extraordinary amount of attention due to observations that chronic administration of many drugs of abuse reduces D2 DA receptor binding in the striatum, suggesting that drug use produces these changes [6]. However, other lines of evidence suggest that D2 DA receptor expression may also correspond to a vulnerability factor. Thus, non-addicted individuals that reported higher drug “liking” scores for methylphenidate also had lower levels of D2 DA receptors within the striatum [7]. Using an animal model, it was observed that over-expressing the D2 DA receptor in the ventral striatum decreases cocaine self-administration [9]. These findings suggest that expression of D2 DA receptors may predict future cocaine use, although neither study address how the sensitivity of the D2 DA receptor may correspond with the responsiveness to psychostimulants.
There are several lines of evidence suggesting that the expression levels of metabotropic receptors can be dissociated from the sensitivity of the receptor to initiate intracellular signaling and influence cellular activity. For example, dissociation was observed in rats following a binge-like administration of cocaine. Thus, decreases in D2 DA receptor Bmax were observed suggesting a decrease in D2 DA receptor expression following binge cocaine administration, while concomitant increases in G protein activation were observed in response to D2 DA receptor stimulation in these same animals [10]. This corresponds with the notion that cocaine self-administration increases the expression of high affinity D2 DA receptors without necessarily influencing the overall expression of D2 DA receptors [11]. Our studies suggest that individual differences in the behavioral sensitivity to D2 DA receptor stimulation predict the responsiveness to cocaine-induced locomotion, reward and reinforcement. Specifically, animals with higher D2 DA receptor behavioral sensitivity, whether it is because of greater expression of high affinity D2 DA receptors, enhanced G protein activation or another cellular mechanism, predisposes animals to greater cocaine sensitivity, reward and reinforcement. It remains undetermined whether HD2 and LD2 rats differ in the expression of D2 DA receptors and/or G protein activation.
Investigating individual differences as a predictor of drug sensitivity, reward and development of addictive-like behavioral changes has been a long-standing approach to determine vulnerability factors. One of the most established animals models utilizes the habituation response to a novel environment to classify animals as either low or high responders (LR or HR, respectively; [26]). In this model, HR rats exhibit a greater locomotor response to acute cocaine and more readily self-administer low doses of psychostimulants compared to LR rats [26], [27], [35], [36]. Interestingly, HR and LR rats also display differences in D2 DA receptor expression where HR rats have decreased Bmax of 3H-raclopride binding and in D2 DA receptor mRNA in the nucleus accumbens [37]. These differences are not reflected in the behavioral sensitivity to D2 DA receptor stimulation since we did not observed differences between HR and LR rats in quinpirole-induced locomotion confirming previous results [38]. In contrast, an analogous study where rats were selectively bred for differences in responsiveness to novelty, high novelty responders displayed a greater proportion of high affinity D2 receptors [39], [40]. Rats bred for high novelty responsiveness also displayed greater quinpirole sensitivity, increased responsiveness to cocaine-related cues and enhanced behavioral disinhibition, findings that are akin to some of our observations. It is unclear whether the differences between HR and LR rats in D2 DA receptor expression reflect pre-synaptic or post-synaptic changes or changes in both populations of D2 DA receptors. One study reports that HR rats possess subsensitivity of D2 autoreceptors in the ventral tegmental area, however it is unknown whether the sensitivity of post-synaptic D2 DA receptors in the striatal terminal regions is different between the HR and LR rats [41]. Given some of the inconsistencies in our observations and previous observations we suspect that our D2 DA receptor group characterization likely corresponds with mechanisms distinct from generalized locomotor responses to novelty and exploratory behaviors.
Another, more recently developed animal model of individual differences utilizes the initial locomotor response to cocaine to determine HCR and LCR rats [28]. This model has established that LCR rats display greater development of cocaine sensitization [29], enhanced conditioned place preference to cocaine [30], and have higher progressive ratio breakpoints than HCR rats [31]. These findings suggest that animals with a low initial response to cocaine may be more vulnerable to cocaine addiction. We observed that HD2 rats have a greater initial response to cocaine, develop cocaine conditioned place preference more readily, and self-administer more cocaine on fixed ratio schedules compared to LD2 rats. In an attempt to relate our findings to those using the HCR/LCR characterization, we re-characterized our animals based on their initial cocaine locomotor response. Using this method, we observed that HCR rats had significantly higher D2 DA receptor sensitivity compared to LCR rats. While these findings are somewhat contradictory since we find that higher D2 DA receptor sensitivity corresponds with behaviors more reminiscent of LCR rats in previous studies (e.g. higher cocaine locomotion, cocaine CPP, increased cocaine self-administration), they are consistent with findings from the Roman high avoidance rat lines where rats that display greater acute locomotor responsiveness self-administer more cocaine [42], [43].
There may be undetermined neurobiological underpinnings that correspond with this discrepancy or it may be a reflection of several experimental differences. First, we did not precisely replicate the published procedures for HCR/LCR characterization. We used a broader characterization of the initial cocaine response. Thus, we collapsed across 2 cocaine doses (5 and 15 mg/kg) and the testing was performed over two hours. This is substantially different than the 30-minute assessment following 10 mg/kg cocaine that was used in previous HCR/LCR studies. Second, the cocaine locomotor testing was performed after the initial quinpirole sensitivity assessment in the same locomotor activity chambers. It is unclear how this experience may have confounded the subsequent cocaine locomotor testing. Finally, we used different procedures in assessing conditioned place preference (ip vs iv cocaine injections) and our self-administration studies were performed after substantial sucrose self-administration. In fact, another recent study utilizing food training prior to cocaine self-administration observed effects more reminiscent of our findings suggesting that this may be an important procedural consideration [44]. In all, these procedural differences may impair our ability to directly compare our studies with those using the HCR/LCR characterization.
Regardless, enhanced initial sensitivity to D2 DA receptor stimulation may reflect a vulnerability factor that contributes to increased psychostimulant use. Our observations exploit differences in D2 DA receptor sensitivities in an outbred, drug-naïve population of rats. It is possible that genetic or environmental factors could influence D2 DA receptor sensitivity rendering some individuals vulnerable or resistant to the behavioral effects of psychostimulants. For example, rearing conditions and social hierarchies have been shown to influence the expression of D2 DA receptors. Isolation housing is associated with decreased D2 DA receptor expression [45], although others report no change in receptor expression and no change in the behavioral sensitivity of D2 DA receptors [46]. In socially housed animals, social dominance can influence the expression of D2 DA receptors where dominant animals display increased D2 DA receptor expression and are resistant to cocaine self-administration [47], [48]. Given that our animals were individually housed, social hierarchies were likely not a contributing factor, although early life social and/or stressful experiences may have impacted D2 DA receptor sensitivities [49], [50], [51], [52], [53], [54], [55].
In summary, we demonstrate that rats with a high initial sensitivity to the locomotor effects of D2 DA receptor stimulation, HD2 rats, correspond with greater sensitivity to cocaine locomotor sensitivity, cocaine reward, and cocaine taking compared with LD2 rats having low initial sensitivity to the locomotor effects produced by D2 DA receptor stimulation. This is the first demonstration that D2 DA receptor sensitivity is a phenotype representing higher susceptibility to cocaine use, given the exacerbation of cocaine’s behavioral effects. Future studies will be aimed at identifying whether D2 DA receptor sensitivity is associated with greater development of behavioral sensitization and cocaine dependence phenotypes as well as associated alterations within the neurobiology of the mesocorticolimbic DA system.
Supporting Information
Figure_S1.tif
- 1 / 2
Distribution of quinpirole-induced locomotion in one cohort of animals. (A) Distribution of locomotor activity scores (beam breaks/hr) during the ascending within-session quinpirole dose response testing. Dark gray horizontal lines within the data clusters depict the median score at each dose. (B) Distribution of the calculated area under the curve (AUC) score for each animal across the three quinpirole doses. The dark gray filled data point and the dotted line represent the median score (M = 15460).
Distribution of quinpirole-induced locomotion in one cohort of animals. (A) Distribution of locomotor activity scores (beam breaks/hr) during the ascending within-session quinpirole dose response testing. Dark gray horizontal lines within the data clusters depict the median score at each dose. (B) Distribution of the calculated area under the curve (AUC) score for each animal across the three quinpirole doses. The dark gray filled data point and the dotted line represent the median score (M = 15460).
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078258.s001
(TIF)
LD2 and HD2 groups did not differ in their D2 dopamine autoreceptor sensitivity. (A) Distribution of the calculated scores (% Baseline) for 0.1 mg/kg quinpirole within the LD2 and HD2 groups. Baseline activity corresponds with saline-induced locomotor activity the hour prior to 0.1 mg/kg quinpirole administration in the within session dose response testing procedure. (B) Group averages (± sem) for the D2 autoreceptor sensitivity scores revealed not significant group differences.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078258.s002
(TIF)
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: RKB KEM. Performed the experiments: KEM. Analyzed the data: RKB. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: RKB KEM. Wrote the paper: RKB.
References
- 1. Wagner FA, Anthony JC (2002) From first drug use to drug dependence; developmental periods of risk for dependence upon marijuana, cocaine, and alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 26: 479–488. doi: 10.1016/s0893-133x(01)00367-0
- 2. Piazza PV, Le Moal ML (1996) Pathophysiological basis of vulnerability to drug abuse: role of an interaction between stress, glucocorticoids, and dopaminergic neurons. Annual review of pharmacology and toxicology 36: 359–378. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pa.36.040196.002043
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- View Article
- PubMed/NCBI
- Google Scholar
- 3. Swanson LW (1982) The projections of the ventral tegmental area and adjacent regions: a combined fluorescent retrograde tracer and immunofluorescence study in the rat. Brain research bulletin 9: 321–353. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(82)90145-9
- 4. Ritz MC, Lamb RJ, Goldberg SR, Kuhar MJ (1987) Cocaine receptors on dopamine transporters are related to self-administration of cocaine. Science 237: 1219–1223. doi: 10.1126/science.2820058
- 5. Anderson SM, Pierce RC (2005) Cocaine-induced alterations in dopamine receptor signaling: implications for reinforcement and reinstatement. Pharmacol Ther 106: 389–403. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2004.12.004
- 6. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Baler R, Telang F (2009) Imaging dopamine’s role in drug abuse and addiction. Neuropharmacology 56 Suppl 13–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.05.022
- 7. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gatley SJ, et al. (1999) Prediction of reinforcing responses to psychostimulants in humans by brain dopamine D2 receptor levels. The American journal of psychiatry 156: 1440–1443.
- 8. Caine SB, Negus SS, Mello NK, Patel S, Bristow L, et al. (2002) Role of dopamine D2-like receptors in cocaine self-administration: studies with D2 receptor mutant mice and novel D2 receptor antagonists. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 22: 2977–2988.
- 9. Thanos PK, Michaelides M, Umegaki H, Volkow ND (2008) D2R DNA transfer into the nucleus accumbens attenuates cocaine self-administration in rats. Synapse 62: 481–486. doi: 10.1002/syn.20523
- 10. Bailey A, Metaxas A, Yoo JH, McGee T, Kitchen I (2008) Decrease of D2 receptor binding but increase in D2-stimulated G-protein activation, dopamine transporter binding and behavioural sensitization in brains of mice treated with a chronic escalating dose ‘binge’ cocaine administration paradigm. Eur J Neurosci 28: 759–770. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06369.x
- 11. Briand LA, Flagel SB, Seeman P, Robinson TE (2008) Cocaine self-administration produces a persistent increase in dopamine D2 High receptors. European neuropsychopharmacology : the journal of the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology 18: 551–556. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.01.002
- 12. Bachtell RK, Choi KH, Simmons DL, Falcon E, Monteggia LM, et al. (2008) Role of GluR1 expression in nucleus accumbens neurons in cocaine sensitization and cocaine-seeking behavior. Eur J Neurosci 27: 2229–2240. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06199.x
- 13. Collins GT, Truong YN, Levant B, Chen J, Wang S, et al. (2011) Behavioral sensitization to cocaine in rats: evidence for temporal differences in dopamine D3 and D2 receptor sensitivity. Psychopharmacology 215: 609–620. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2154-7
- 14. Edwards S, Whisler KN, Fuller DC, Orsulak PJ, Self DW (2007) Addiction-related alterations in D1 and D2 dopamine receptor behavioral responses following chronic cocaine self-administration. Neuropsychopharmacology 32: 354–366. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301062
- 15. Ujike H, Akiyama K, Otsuki S (1990) D-2 but not D-1 dopamine agonists produce augmented behavioral response in rats after subchronic treatment with methamphetamine or cocaine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 102: 459–464. doi: 10.1007/bf02247125
- 16. Bachtell RK, Whisler K, Karanian D, Self DW (2005) Effects of intra-nucleus accumbens shell administration of dopamine agonists and antagonists on cocaine-taking and cocaine-seeking behaviors in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 183: 41–53. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0133-1
- 17. De Vries TJ, Schoffelmeer AN, Binnekade R, Vanderschuren LJ (1999) Dopaminergic mechanisms mediating the incentive to seek cocaine and heroin following long-term withdrawal of IV drug self-administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 143: 254–260. doi: 10.1007/s002130050944
- 18. Dias C, Lachize S, Boilet V, Huitelec E, Cador M (2004) Differential effects of dopaminergic agents on locomotor sensitisation and on the reinstatement of cocaine-seeking and food-seeking behaviour. Psychopharmacology 175: 105–115. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1839-1
- 19. Khroyan TV, Barrett-Larimore RL, Rowlett JK, Spealman RD (2000) Dopamine D1- and D2-like receptor mechanisms in relapse to cocaine-seeking behavior: Effects of selective antagonists and agonists. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 294: 680–687.
- 20. Schmidt HD, Pierce RC (2006) Cooperative activation of D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens shell is required for the reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in the rat. Neuroscience 142: 451–461. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.004
- 21. Self DW, Barnhart WJ, Lehman DA, Nestler EJ (1996) Opposite modulation of cocaine-seeking behavior by D1- and D2-like dopamine receptor agonists. Science 271: 1586–1589. doi: 10.1126/science.271.5255.1586
- 22. O’Neill CE, LeTendre ML, Bachtell RK (2012) Adenosine A2A receptors in the nucleus accumbens bi-directionally alter cocaine seeking in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 37: 1245–1256. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.312
- 23. White FJ, Wang RY (1986) Electrophysiological evidence for the existence of both D-1 and D-2 dopamine receptors in the rat nucleus accumbens. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 6: 274–280.
- 24. Hu XT, Wang RY (1988) Disinhibition of nucleus accumbens neurons by the dopamine D2 receptor agonist LY-141865: prevented by 6-OHDA pretreatment. Brain research 444: 389–393. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90953-5
- 25. Eilam D, Szechtman H (1989) Biphasic effect of D-2 agonist quinpirole on locomotion and movements. European journal of pharmacology 161: 151–157. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(89)90837-6
- 26. Piazza PV, Deminiere JM, Le Moal M, Simon H (1989) Factors that predict individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Science 245: 1511–1513. doi: 10.1126/science.2781295
- 27. Piazza PV, Deroche-Gamonent V, Rouge-Pont F, Le Moal M (2000) Vertical shifts in self-administration dose-response functions predict a drug-vulnerable phenotype predisposed to addiction. J Neurosci 20: 4226–4232.
- 28. Gulley JM, Hoover BR, Larson GA, Zahniser NR (2003) Individual differences in cocaine-induced locomotor activity in rats: behavioral characteristics, cocaine pharmacokinetics, and the dopamine transporter. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 28: 2089–2101. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300279
- 29. Sabeti J, Gerhardt GA, Zahniser NR (2003) Individual differences in cocaine-induced locomotor sensitization in low and high cocaine locomotor-responding rats are associated with differential inhibition of dopamine clearance in nucleus accumbens. The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics 305: 180–190. doi: 10.1124/jpet.102.047258
- 30. Allen RM, Everett CV, Nelson AM, Gulley JM, Zahniser NR (2007) Low and high locomotor responsiveness to cocaine predicts intravenous cocaine conditioned place preference in male Sprague-Dawley rats. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior 86: 37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.12.005
- 31. Mandt BH, Schenk S, Zahniser NR, Allen RM (2008) Individual differences in cocaine-induced locomotor activity in male Sprague-Dawley rats and their acquisition of and motivation to self-administer cocaine. Psychopharmacology 201: 195–202. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1265-x
- 32. Sokoloff P, Giros B, Martres MP, Bouthenet ML, Schwartz JC (1990) Molecular cloning and characterization of a novel dopamine receptor (D3) as a target for neuroleptics. Nature 347: 146–151. doi: 10.1038/347146a0
- 33. Kostrzewa RM, Brus R (1991) Is dopamine-agonist induced yawning behavior a D3 mediated event? Life sciences 48: PL129. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(91)90619-m
- 34. Kurashima M, Yamada K, Nagashima M, Shirakawa K, Furukawa T (1995) Effects of putative dopamine D3 receptor agonists, 7-OH-DPAT, and quinpirole, on yawning, stereotypy, and body temperature in rats. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior 52: 503–508. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)00103-4
- 35. Deminiere JM, Piazza PV, Le Moal M, Simon H (1989) Experimental approach to individual vulnerability to psychostimulant addiction. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 13: 141–147. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(89)80023-5
- 36. Hooks MS, Jones GH, Smith AD, Neill DB, Justice JB Jr (1991) Individual differences in locomotor activity and sensitization. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior 38: 467–470. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90308-o
- 37. Hooks MS, Juncos JL, Justice JB Jr, Meiergerd SM, Povlock SL, et al. (1994) Individual locomotor response to novelty predicts selective alterations in D1 and D2 receptors and mRNAs. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 14: 6144–6152.
- 38. Hooks MS, Jones DN, Holtzman SG, Juncos JL, Kalivas PW, et al. (1994) Individual differences in behavior following amphetamine, GBR-12909, or apomorphine but not SKF-38393 or quinpirole. Psychopharmacology 116: 217–225. doi: 10.1007/bf02245065
- 39. Flagel SB, Robinson TE, Clark JJ, Clinton SM, Watson SJ, et al. (2010) An animal model of genetic vulnerability to behavioral disinhibition and responsiveness to reward-related cues: implications for addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 35: 388–400. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.142
- 40. Seeman P, Weinshenker D, Quirion R, Srivastava LK, Bhardwaj SK, et al. (2005) Dopamine supersensitivity correlates with D2High states, implying many paths to psychosis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 3513–3518. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409766102
- 41. Marinelli M, White FJ (2000) Enhanced vulnerability to cocaine self-administration is associated with elevated impulse activity of midbrain dopamine neurons. J Neurosci 20: 8876–8885.
- 42. Fattore L, Piras G, Corda MG, Giorgi O (2009) The Roman high- and low-avoidance rat lines differ in the acquisition, maintenance, extinction, and reinstatement of intravenous cocaine self-administration. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 34: 1091–1101. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.43
- 43. Giorgi O, Piras G, Corda MG (2007) The psychogenetically selected Roman high- and low-avoidance rat lines: a model to study the individual vulnerability to drug addiction. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 31: 148–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.07.008
- 44. Schramm-Sapyta NL, Cauley MC, Stangl DK, Glowacz S, Stepp KA, et al. (2011) Role of individual and developmental differences in voluntary cocaine intake in rats. Psychopharmacology 215: 493–504. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2216-5
- 45. Rilke O, May T, Oehler J, Wolffgramm J (1995) Influences of housing conditions and ethanol intake on binding characteristics of D2, 5-HT1A, and benzodiazepine receptors of rats. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior 52: 23–28. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)00093-c
- 46. Del Arco A, Zhu S, Terasmaa A, Mohammed AH, Fuxe K (2004) Hyperactivity to novelty induced by social isolation is not correlated with changes in D2 receptor function and binding in striatum. Psychopharmacology 171: 148–155. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1578-8
- 47. Grant KA, Shively CA, Nader MA, Ehrenkaufer RL, Line SW, et al. (1998) Effect of social status on striatal dopamine D2 receptor binding characteristics in cynomolgus monkeys assessed with positron emission tomography. Synapse 29: 80–83. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-2396(199805)29:1<80::aid-syn7>3.0.co;2-7
- 48. Morgan D, Grant KA, Gage HD, Mach RH, Kaplan JR, et al. (2002) Social dominance in monkeys: dopamine D2 receptors and cocaine self-administration. Nat Neurosci 5: 169–174. doi: 10.1038/nn798
- 49. Papp M, Muscat R, Willner P (1993) Subsensitivity to rewarding and locomotor stimulant effects of a dopamine agonist following chronic mild stress. Psychopharmacology 110: 152–158. doi: 10.1007/bf02246965
- 50. Papp M, Klimek V, Willner P (1994) Parallel changes in dopamine D2 receptor binding in limbic forebrain associated with chronic mild stress-induced anhedonia and its reversal by imipramine. Psychopharmacology 115: 441–446. doi: 10.1007/bf02245566
- 51. Puglisi-Allegra S, Kempf E, Schleef C, Cabib S (1991) Repeated stressful experiences differently affect brain dopamine receptor subtypes. Life sciences 48: 1263–1268. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(91)90521-c
- 52. Henry C, Guegant G, Cador M, Arnauld E, Arsaut J, et al. (1995) Prenatal stress in rats facilitates amphetamine-induced sensitization and induces long-lasting changes in dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens. Brain research 685: 179–186. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00430-x
- 53. Cabib S, Giardino L, Calza L, Zanni M, Mele A, et al. (1998) Stress promotes major changes in dopamine receptor densities within the mesoaccumbens and nigrostriatal systems. Neuroscience 84: 193–200. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00468-5
- 54. Dziedzicka-Wasylewska M, Willner P, Papp M (1997) Changes in dopamine receptor mRNA expression following chronic mild stress and chronic antidepressant treatment. Behavioural pharmacology 8: 607–618. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199711000-00017
- 55. Carr KD, Kim GY, Cabeza de Vaca S (2001) Rewarding and locomotor-activating effects of direct dopamine receptor agonists are augmented by chronic food restriction in rats. Psychopharmacology 154: 420–428. doi: 10.1007/s002130000674