Food reward, hyperphagia, and obesity (2011)

. 2011 Jun; 300(6): R1266–R1277.

Published online 2011 Mar 16. doi:  10.1152/ajpregu.00028.2011

PMCID: PMC3119156

Abstract

Given the unabated obesity problem, there is increasing appreciation of expressions like “my eyes are bigger than my stomach,” and recent studies in rodents and humans suggest that dysregulated brain reward pathways may be contributing not only to drug addiction but also to increased intake of palatable foods and ultimately obesity. After describing recent progress in revealing the neural pathways and mechanisms underlying food reward and the attribution of incentive salience by internal state signals, we analyze the potentially circular relationship between palatable food intake, hyperphagia, and obesity. Are there preexisting individual differences in reward functions at an early age, and could they be responsible for development of obesity later in life? Does repeated exposure to palatable foods set off a cascade of sensitization as in drug and alcohol addiction? Are reward functions altered by secondary effects of the obese state, such as increased signaling through inflammatory, oxidative, and mitochondrial stress pathways? Answering these questions will significantly impact prevention and treatment of obesity and its ensuing comorbidities as well as eating disorders and drug and alcohol addiction.

Keywords: palatability, food addiction, liking, wanting, motivation, reinforcement, neuroimaging, leptin, insulin, body weight, weight loss

the current obesity epidemic is best explained as a mismatch between the modern environment/lifestyle and biological response patterns that evolved in a scarce environment. Biological traits like strong attraction to food and food cues, slow satiety mechanisms, and high metabolic efficiency, advantageous for survival in a scarce environment, seem now to be our worst enemies when it comes to resisting an abundance of food (, ). Food intake and energy expenditure are thought to be controlled by complex, redundant, and distributed neural systems, likely involving thousands of genes and reflecting the fundamental biological importance of adequate nutrient supply and energy balance (, ). There has been much progress in identifying the important role of the hypothalamus and areas in the brainstem in the various hormonal and neural mechanisms by which the brain informs itself about availability of ingested and stored nutrients and, in turn, generates behavioral, autonomic, and endocrine output (, ) (Fig. 1). Some of the genes involved in this homeostatic regulator are crucial for energy balance as manifested in the well-known monogenic obesity models such as leptin-deficiency (). However, it can be clearly demonstrated that much larger portions of the nervous system of animals and humans, including cortex, basal ganglia, and the limbic system, are concerned with the procurement of food as a basic and evolutionarily conserved survival mechanism to defend body weight (). By forming representations and reward expectancies through processes of learning and memory, these systems likely evolved to engage powerful motivations and drives for guaranteed supply with and ingestion of beneficial foods from a sparse and often hostile environment. Now these systems are simply overwhelmed with an abundance of food and food cues that are no longer contested by predators and interrupted by famines (). Regrettably, the anatomy, chemistry, and functions of these elaborate neural systems and their interactions with the homeostatic regulator in the hypothalamus are poorly understood. These systems are directly and primarily involved in the interactions of the modern environment and lifestyle with the human body. They are no less physiological than metabolic regulatory mechanisms that have attracted most of the research during the past 15 years.

Fig. 1. 

Schematic flow diagram showing the relationship between the classical homeostatic regulator (dark gray boxes) and neural systems involved in reward, cognitive, and executive functions (light gray boxes). Note that humoral (broken lines with open arrows)

This review aims to provide a brief overview of the current concepts of neural control of food reward and the possible involvement of abnormal food reward processing in causing hyperphagia and obesity and potential maladaptive effects of palatable diets on reward processing. Two excellent recent reviews have discussed the relation of obesity to food reward from mainly the clinical and psychological perspective (, ). Here, we focus on neural correlates of reward, the interactions between reward and homeostatic functions, and the disturbance of this relationship in obesity (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. 

Relationship between metabolic and hedonic controls of food intake and energy balance. The metabolic consequences of food are regulated by homeostatic functions and the hedonic consequences by reward functions. Hedonic and metabolic consequences are interdependent

Glossary

Definitions were adopted from Berridge et al. ():

Food Reward

A composite process that contains “liking” (hedonic impact), “wanting” (incentive motivation), and learning (associations and predictions) as major components. Normally all occur together, but the three psychological components have separable brain systems that permit dissociation among them in some conditions.

“Liking” (with quotation marks)

An objective hedonic reaction detected in behavior or neural signals and generated chiefly by subcortical brain systems. A “liking” reaction to sweetness produces conscious pleasure by recruiting additional brain circuits, but a core “liking” reaction can sometimes occur without subjective pleasure.

Liking (without quotation marks)

The everyday sense of the word as a subjective conscious feeling of pleasurable niceness.

“Wanting” (with quotation marks)

Incentive salience or motivation for reward typically triggered by reward-related cues. Attribution of incentive salience to the representations makes a cue and its reward more attractive, sought after, and likely to be consumed. Brain mesolimbic systems, especially those involving dopamine, are especially important to “wanting.” Ordinarily “wanting” occurs together with other reward components of “liking” and learning and with subjective desires but can be dissociated both from other components and subjective desire under some conditions.

Wanting (without quotation marks)

A conscious, cognitive desire for a declarative goal in the ordinary sense of the word wanting. This cognitive form of wanting involves additional cortical brain mechanisms beyond the mesolimbic systems that mediate “wanting” as incentive salience.

Other definitions:

Palatable/Palatability

Foods that are acceptable or agreeable to the palate or taste. Synonyms include tasty or delectable. Generally, palatable foods are also energy-dense and include high-fat, high-sugar, or both, foods.

Sensory-Specific Satiety

The phenomenon wherein hungry animals satiate on one food and do not partake when offered the same food again; the same animals offered a second novel food consume another meal.

Metabolic Hunger

Hunger driven by metabolic need, mediated by endogenous signals of nutrient depletion.

Hedonic Hunger

Eating driven by other than metabolic need, such as external cues.

Hedonic Consequences of Food

The many pleasures of eating.

Eating is typically experienced as pleasurable and rewarding, and it has been speculated that the inherent pleasantness of eating has evolved to provide the necessary motivation to engage in this crucial behavior in adverse and hostile environments (). Thus, food is a powerful natural reinforcer that out competes most other behaviors, particularly when an individual is metabolically hungry. Ingestive behavior is not limited to the act of eating, but consists of preparatory, consummatory, and postconsummatory phases (). Hedonic evaluation and reward processing is carried out in each of these three phases of ingestive behavior and critically determines their outcome.

In the preparatory phase, before any oral contact is made with food, reward expectancy plays a pivotal role. This phase can be further divided into an initiation phase (switch of attention from another behavior) a procurement phase (planning, foraging), and an appetitive phase (seeing and smelling food). The initiation phase is the key process in which a choice, selection, or decision is made to pursue a particular goal-directed activity and not another one. The decision-making process responsible for switching attention is central to the modern field of neuroeconomics, and reward expectancy is perhaps the main factor determining the outcome of this process. Research suggests that to make this choice, the brain uses representations of reward-expectancy and effort/risk requirement from prior experiences to optimize cost/benefit (, , , , ). Thus, the decision to pursue this new goal largely depends on expecting but not actually consuming the reward. The time period between making a decision and actually being able to consume the reward is the procurement phase. This phase used to be quite long in our human ancestors and in today’s free-living animals, as e.g., illustrated by the Canadian mountain goat descending from higher elevations to the river bed over a hundred miles to satisfy its salt appetite. Reward expectancy appears to be the main driver to keep focused during this journey. During the appetitive phase, immediate sensory attributes of the goal object such as seeing, smelling, and ultimately tasting the first bite of the food start to provide the first feedback to its predicted reward value and may acutely enhance its motivating power. This amplification of appetite is reflected by the generation of cephalic phase responses, anecdotally known by the French as l’appetit vient en mangeant (appetite grows with the first bites). The first bite is also the last chance to reject food if it does not fulfill expectations or is even toxic.

The consummatory phase (meal) starts when, based on the first bite, the initial reward expectancy is confirmed or surpassed. During eating, immediate, direct pleasure is derived from mainly gustatory and olfactory sensations, driving consumption throughout the meal until satiation signals dominate (). The length of the consummatory phase is highly variable as it takes only a few minutes to devour a hamburger, but may take hours to savor a five-course meal. During such longer meals, ingested food increasingly engages postoral reward processes that interact with oral reward.

The postconsummatory phase starts at meal termination and lasts all the way to the next ingestive bout. This phase is probably the most complex and least understood phase of ingestive behavior in terms of reward processing, although the mechanisms of satiation and satiety have been exhaustively studied and a long list of satiety factors have been identified. As mentioned above, nutrient sensors in the gastrointestinal tract and elsewhere in the body appear to also contribute to the generation of food reward during and after a meal (). The same taste receptors found in the oral cavity are also expressed in gut epithelial cells () and in the hypothalamus (). But even when all taste processing is eliminated by genetic manipulation, mice still learn to prefer sugar over water, suggesting the generation of food reward by processes of glucose utilization (). Rather than the acute pleasure of tasty food in the mouth, there is a general feeling of satisfaction that lingers on long after termination, and most likely contributes to the reinforcing power of a meal. Furthermore, in humans, meals are often embedded in enjoyable social interactions and a pleasant ambiance. Finally, knowledge that eating particular foods or reducing caloric intake will pay off by being healthier and living longer can generate yet another form of happiness or reward.

Thus, a variety of sensory stimuli and emotional states or feelings with vastly different temporal profiles make up the rewarding experience of eating, and the underlying neural functions are only beginning to be understood.

Neural mechanisms of food reward functions: liking and wanting.

Just as there is no hunger center, there is no pleasure center in the brain. Given the complex involvement of pleasure and reward in ingestive (and other) motivated behaviors as outlined above, it is clear that multiple neural systems are involved. Neural systems activated by thinking about a favorite dish, savoring a candy in the mouth, or leaning back after a satiating meal, are likely very different, although they may contain common elements. To identify these differences and common elements is the ultimate goal of researchers in the field of ingestive behavior.

Perhaps the most easily accessible process is the acute pleasure generated by a candy in the mouth. Even in the fruit fly with its primitive nervous system, stimulation of gustatory neurons with sugar activated, while stimulation with a bitter substance inhibited, a pair of motor neurons in the subesophageal ganglion, leading to either vigorous ingestion or rejection (), adding to the mounting evidence that taste evolved as a hardwired system telling the animal to either accept or reject certain foods. In mice with transgenic expression of the receptor for an ordinarily tasteless ligand in either sweet or bitter taste receptor cells, stimulation with the ligand produced either strong attraction or avoidance of sweet solutions, respectively (). Most remarkably, quinine, a cognate bitter ligand, produced strong attraction in mice with expression of a bitter receptor in sweet-sensing taste receptor cells (). These findings suggest that the most primitive form of liking and disliking may already be inherent to components of the peripheral gustatory pathways. As demonstrated in the decerebrate rat () and anencephalic baby (), expression of the characteristic happy face when tasting sweets (, ) appears to be neurologically organized within the brainstem, suggesting that the forebrain is not necessary for expression of this most primitive form of core “liking” (). In mammals, the caudal brainstem is the equivalent of the subesophageal ganglion, where direct sensory feedback from the tongue and the gut are integrated into basic motor patterns of ingestion (, ). Thus, this basic brainstem circuitry appears to be able to recognize the usefulness and perhaps pleasantness of a taste stimulus and initiate appropriate behavioral responses.

However, even if some of this primitive taste-guided reflexive behavior is organized within the brainstem, it is clear that the brainstem circuits are normally not acting in isolation, but are intimately communicating with the forebrain. Even in Drosophila, the taste-specific receptor cells do not directly synapse on motor neurons responsible for the taste-guided behavioral output (), leaving plenty of opportunities for modulatory influences from other areas of the nervous system. Clearly, for the full sensory impact of palatable food, and the subjective feeling of pleasure in humans, taste is integrated with other sensory modalities such as smell and mouth feel in forebrain areas including the amygdala, as well as primary and higher-order sensory cortical areas, including the insular and orbitofrontal cortex, to form sensory representations of particular foods (, , , , , , ). The exact neural pathways through which such sensory percepts or representations lead to the generation of subjective pleasure (Berridge’s “liking”, see Glossary) are not clear. Neuroimaging studies in humans suggest that pleasure, as measured by subjective ratings, is computed within portions of the orbitofrontal and perhaps insular cortex (, ).

In animals, only the subconscious components of pleasure (Berridge’s core “liking”) and aversion are experimentally accessible, and one of the few specific test paradigms is measurement of positive and negative orofacial expressions when tasting pleasurable (typically sweet) or aversive stimuli (). Using this method, Berridge and colleagues (, ) have demonstrated narrowly circumscribed, μ-opioid receptor-mediated pleasure (“liking”) hotspots in the nucleus accumbens shell and ventral pallidum. We have recently demonstrated that nucleus accumbens injection of a μ-opioid receptor antagonist transiently suppressed such sucrose-evoked positive hedonic orofacial reactions (). Together the findings suggest that endogenous μ-opioid signaling in the nucleus accumbens (ventral striatum) is critically involved in the expression of “liking.” Because the measured behavioral output is organized within the brainstem, the ventral striatal “liking” hotspot must somehow communicate with this basic reflex circuitry, but the pathways of communication are unclear.

One of the key questions is how motivation to obtain a reward is translated into action (). In most instances, motivation comes to fruition by going for something that has generated pleasure in the past, or in other words by wanting what is liked. Dopamine signaling within the mesolimbic dopamine projection system appears to be a crucial component of this process. Phasic activity of dopamine neuron projections from the ventral tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens in the ventral striatum are specifically involved in the decision-making process during the preparatory (appetitive) phase of ingestive behavior (, ). In addition, when palatable foods such as sucrose are actually consumed, a sustained and sweetness-dependent increase occurs in nucleus accumbens dopamine levels and turnover (, , ). Dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens thus appears to play a role in both the preparatory and consummatory phases of an ingestive bout. The nucleus accumbens shell is thereby part of a neural loop including the lateral hypothalamus and the ventral tegmental area, with orexin neurons playing a key role (, , , , , , , ). This loop is likely important for the attribution of incentive salience to goal objects by metabolic state signals available to the lateral hypothalamus, as discussed below.

In summary, although there have been excellent recent attempts to separate its components, the functional concept and neural circuitry underlying food reward is still poorly defined. Specifically, it is not well understood how reward, generated during anticipation, consummation, and satiation, are computed and integrated. Future research with modern neuroimaging techniques in humans and invasive neurochemical analyses in animals will be necessary for a more complete understanding. Perhaps the most important processing step in the translation of such sensory representations into actions is the attribution of what Berridge calls “incentive salience.” This mechanism allows a starving animal to know it needs calories or a salt-depleted organism to know it needs salt. The modulation of hedonic processes by the metabolic state is discussed below.

Metabolic State Modulates Hedonic Processing

The metabolic consequences of ingested food are defined here in terms of their input of energy and their effects on body composition, particularly increased fat accretion as in obesity. Together with the control of energy expenditure, these functions are known as homeostatic regulation of body weight and adiposity (Fig. 1). It has long been known that metabolic hunger increases motivation to find food and to eat, but the neural mechanisms involved were obscure. Given that the hypothalamus was recognized as the epicenter of homeostatic regulation, it was assumed that the metabolic hunger signal originates in this brain area and propagates through neural projections to other areas important for the organization of goal-directed behavior. Thus, when leptin was discovered, researchers were initially content to limit their search for leptin receptors to the hypothalamus, and the initial localization to the arcuate nucleus further propagated the hypothalamocentric view (, ). However, during the last few years it became increasingly clear that leptin and the plethora of other metabolic signals not only act on the hypothalamus, but on a large number of brain systems.

Modulation via the hypothalamus.

Within the hypothalamus, the arcuate nucleus with its neuropeptide Y and proopiomelanocortin neurons had been originally thought to play an exclusive role in integrating metabolic signals. But clearly, leptin receptors are located in other hypothalamic areas such as the ventromedial, dorsomedial, and premammillary nuclei, as well as the lateral and perifornical areas where they likely contribute to leptin’s effects on food intake and energy expenditure (, ). It has long been known that electrical stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus elicits food intake and that rats learn quickly to self-administer electrical stimulation (, ). Metabolic signals modulate the stimulation threshold for lateral hypothalamic elicited self-stimulation and feeding (, , , , , ). Recent investigations show that lateral hypothalamic neurons expressing orexin (, ) and other transmitters such as neurotensin (, ) provide modulatory input to midbrain dopamine neurons well known to be crucial players in translating motivation into action (, , , , , , , , ). Orexin neurons can integrate various metabolic state signals such as leptin, insulin, and glucose (, , , , ). In addition to midbrain dopamine neurons, orexin neurons project widely within both forebrain and hindbrain. In particular, a hypothalamic-thalamic-striatal loop involving orexin projections to the paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus and cholinergic striatal interneurons (), and orexin projections to oromotor and autonomic motor areas in the caudal brainstem (). All of these strategic projections put lateral hypothalamic orexin neurons in an ideal position to link internal needs with environmental possibilities to make optimal adaptive choices.

Modulation of “wanting” through the mesolimbic dopamine system.

Considerable evidence has recently accumulated for a direct modulation of midbrain dopamine neurons by metabolic state signals. After the initial demonstration that leptin and insulin injections directly into this brain area suppressed expression of food-conditioned place preference (), other studies demonstrated that such leptin injections decreased dopamine neuron activity and acutely suppressed food intake, while adenoviral knockdown of leptin receptors specifically in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) resulted in increased sucrose preference and sustained palatable food intake (). In contrast, ghrelin action directly within the VTA appears to activate dopamine neurons, increase accumbens dopamine turnover, and increase food intake (, , ). Together, these findings suggest that part of the orexigenic drive of ghrelin and the anorexigenic drive of leptin is achieved by direct modulation of reward-seeking functions mediated by midbrain dopamine neurons. However, this modulation may be more complex, as leptin-deficient mice (absence of leptin-receptor signaling) exhibit suppressed rather than increased dopamine neuron activity [as expected from the viral knockdown experiments in rats ()], and leptin-replacement therapy restored normal dopamine neuron activity as well as amphetamine-induced locomotor sensitization (). Also, in normal rats, leptin promotes tyrosine hydroxylase activity and amphetamine-mediated dopamine efflux in the nucleus accumbens (, ). This opens up the interesting possibility that a suppressed mesolimbic dopamine signaling system (rather than an overactive one) is associated with development of compensatory hyperphagia and obesity, as proposed by the reward-deficiency hypothesis discussed in the next main section. Under this scenario, leptin would be expected to increase dopamine-signaling efficiency rather than suppress it.

Modulation of “liking” through sensory processing, cortical representation, and cognitive controls.

As elaborated above, food-related visual, olfactory, gustatory, and other information converges in polymodal association and related areas such as the orbitofrontal cortex, insula, and amygdala, where it is thought to form representations of experience with food to guide current and future behavior. Recent studies suggest that the sensitivity of these sensory channels and activity within the orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala, and insula are modulated by metabolic state signals.

In rodents, absence of leptin has been shown to increase and addition of leptin to dampen peripheral taste and olfactory sensitivity (, , ). Leptin may also modulate sensory processing at higher gustatory and olfactory processing steps, as indicated by the presence of leptin receptors and leptin-induced Fos expression in the nucleus of the solitary tract, parabrachial nucleus, olfactory bulb, and insular and piriform cortices of rodents (, , , , ).

In the orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala of monkeys, individual neurons responsive to the taste of specific nutrients such as glucose, amino acids, and fat were modulated by hunger in a sensory-specific manner (, , , ). Similarly, subjective pleasantness in humans was coded by neural activity in the medial orbitofrontal cortex as measured by functional MRI (fMRI) and was subject to sensory-specific satiety, a form of reinforcer devaluation (, , , ).

Also by fMRI measurement, it was shown that taste-induced changes in neuronal activation occurred within several areas of the human insular and orbitofrontal cortex and preferentially in the right hemisphere (). Comparing the fasted vs. fed state, food deprivation increased activation of visual (occipitotemporal cortex) and gustatory (insular cortex) sensory processing areas by the sight and taste of food (). In another study, pictures of food that elicited strong activation of visual and premotor cortex, hippocampus, and hypothalamus under eucaloric conditions, elicited much weaker activation after 2 days of overfeeding (). In a recent study exploring the functional neurological consequences of dieting in obese humans, it was found that after a diet-induced 10% body weight loss, neural changes induced by visual food cues were significantly enhanced in several brain areas dealing with higher-order sensory perception and processing of working memory, including an area in the middle temporal gyrus involved in higher-order visual processing (). Both of these weight loss-induced differences were reversed after leptin treatment, suggesting that low leptin sensitizes brain areas responding to food cues. Neural activation in the nucleus accumbens elicited by visual food stimuli is very high in genetically leptin-deficient adolescents and promptly returns to normal levels upon leptin administration (). In the leptin-deficient state, nucleus accumbens activation was positively correlated with ratings of liking for the food shown in images in both the fasted and fed state. Even foods considered bland under normal conditions (with leptin in the satiated state) were liked very much in the absence of leptin signaling. After leptin treatment in these leptin-deficient patients, and in normal subjects, nucleus accumbens activation was only correlated with ratings of liking in the fasted state ().

Furthermore, neural activity in brain areas thought to be involved in cognitive processing of representations of food such as the amygdala and hippocampal complex is modulated by leptin (, , ) and ghrelin (, , , , , ). Thus, it is quite clear that processes of subconscious hedonic evaluation and subjective experience of pleasantness in animals and humans are modulated by the internal state.

In summary, metabolic state signals impinge on almost every neural process involved in procuring, consuming, and learning about food. It is thus unlikely that the mechanisms attributing incentive salience to appetitive stimuli are exclusively originating from nutrient-sensing areas in the mediobasal hypothalamus. Rather, this life-sustaining process is organized in a redundant and distributed fashion.

Food Reward and Obesity

As schematically depicted in Fig. 2, several potential interactions exist between food reward and obesity. The discussion here will focus on three fundamental mechanisms: 1) genetic and other preexisting differences in reward functions potentially causing obesity; 2) intake of palatable food as an escalating, addictive process leading to obesity; and 3) acceleration of obesity through changes in reward functions induced by secondary effects of the obese state. These mechanisms are not mutually exclusive, and it is highly likely that a combination of all three is operative in most individuals. It is also important to realize that hyperphagia is not always necessary for obesity to develop, as the macronutrient composition of food can independently favor fat deposition.

Do genetic and other preexisting differences in reward functions cause obesity?

One fundamental premise here is that unlimited access to palatable food leads to hedonic overeating and eventually obesity, called the gluttony hypothesis for simplicity. This hypothesis is supported by numerous studies in animals demonstrating increased intake of palatable foods and development of obesity, so called diet-induced obesity (, , , , , , , , ). There are also plenty of human studies showing acute effects of manipulating palatability, variability, and availability of food (, ), although few controlled studies show long-term effects on energy balance (, ).

In its purest form, the gluttony hypothesis does not require reward functions to be abnormal; it only requires the environmental conditions to be abnormal (increased access to palatable foods and exposure to cues). Although environmental pressure undoubtedly pushes the general population to higher food intake and body weight, this simple explanation does not account for the fact that not all subjects exposed to the same toxic environment gain weight. This suggests that preexisting differences make some individuals more vulnerable to the increased availability of palatable food and food cues, and the crucial question is what these differences might be. Here we argue that differences in reward functions are responsible, but it is equally possible that differences in the way the homeostatic system handles hedonic overeating are important. Under this scenario, an individual would show all signs of acute hedonic overeating, but the homeostatic regulator (or other mechanisms causing negative energy balance) would be able to counteract this effect over the long term.

Preexisting differences could be determined by genetic and epigenetic alterations, and by early life experience through developmental programming. Among the 20 or so major genes (clear evidence from at least two independent studies) linked to the development of obesity (), none are directly implicated in known mechanisms of reward functions. However, because the combined effect of these genes only accounts for less than ∼5% of human obesity, it is very likely that many important genes have not yet been discovered, some of which could operate within the reward system.

There is a considerable body of literature demonstrating differences in reward functions between lean and obese animals and humans (, , , ). Such differences could exist before the development of obesity or could be secondary to the obese state, but few studies have attempted to dissociate these two mechanisms. It is also important to note that preexisting differences in reward functions do not automatically result in obesity later in life.

Comparing lean and obese subjects carrying different alleles of either the dopamine D2-receptor or μ-opioid receptor genes does reveal differences in behavioral and neural responses to palatable food (, , , ). In selectively bred lines of obesity-prone and obesity-resistant rats, several differences in mesolimbic dopamine signaling have been reported (, ), but most of these studies used adult, already obese animals. In only one preliminary study was a difference seen at an early age (), so it is not clear whether differences in reward functions are preexisting and genetically determined or acquired by exposure to palatable food stimuli and/or secondary to the obese state. Because obesity-prone rats develop some degree of obesity even on regular chow diet, it is also not clear to what extent the genetic difference depends on availability of palatable diet vs. chow, to be phenotypically expressed (susceptibility genes). Mesolimbic dopamine signaling is also severely suppressed in leptin-deficient ob/ob mice and rescued by systemic leptin replacement (). However, in genetically leptin-deficient humans, neural activity in the nucleus accumbens elicited by viewing pictures of palatable foods was exaggerated in the absence of leptin and abolished after leptin administration (). Furthermore, PET neuroimaging showed reduced dopamine D2-receptor availability mostly in the dorsal and lateral, but not ventral, striatum (). On the basis of this last observation, the reward-deficiency hypothesis was coined, suggesting that increased food intake is an attempt to generate more reward in compensation for reduced mesolimbic dopamine signaling (, , ). Clearly, evidence not confounded by differences in subjects and methodology is needed for clarity in understanding of how mesolimbic dopamine signaling is involved in hyperphagia of palatable food and the development of obesity.

Besides classical genetic, epigenetic and nongenetic mechanisms (, , , , , , , , , ) could also be potentially responsible for differences in neural reward circuitry and reward behaviors at a young age, predisposing to hyperphagia and obesity later in life. Such effects are best demonstrated in genetically identical C57/BL6J inbred mice or identical twins. In one such study, only about half of male C57/BL6J mice became obese on a palatable high-fat diet (), but reward functions were not assessed.

In summary, differences in mesolimbic dopamine signaling are most strongly implicated in altered food anticipatory and consummatory behaviors and obesity. However, it is still unclear to what extent preexisting differences and/or secondary effects determine these behavioral alterations and cause obesity. Only longitudinal studies in genetically defined populations will provide more conclusive answers.

Is repeated exposure to addictive foods changing reward mechanisms and leading to accelerated development of obesity?

There is heated discussion about similarities between food and drug addiction (, , , , , , , , , , ). While the field of drug addiction has a long tradition (e.g., Refs. , ), the concept of food addiction is still not generally accepted, and its behavioral and neurological mechanisms remain obscure. It is well-known that repeated exposure to drugs of abuse causes neuroadaptive changes leading to elevations in reward thresholds (decreased reward) that drive accelerated drug intake (, , , , , ). The question here is whether repeated exposure to palatable food can lead to similar neuroadaptive changes in the food-reward system and behavioral dependence (craving for palatable foods and withdrawal symptoms) and whether this is independent from obesity that typically results after prolonged exposure to palatable foods. The limited information available suggests that repeated sucrose access can upregulate dopamine release () and dopamine transporter (), and change dopamine D1 and D2 receptor availability (, ) in the nucleus accumbens. These changes may be responsible for the observed escalation of sucrose bingeing, cross-sensitization to amphetamine-induced locomotor activity, withdrawal symptoms, such as increased anxiety and depression (), as well as reduced reinforcing efficacy of normal foods (). For nonsweet palatable foods (typically high-fat foods), there is less convincing evidence for development of dependence (, ), although intermittent access to corn oil can stimulate dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens ().

In Wistar rats, exposure to a palatable cafeteria diet led to sustained hyperphagia over 40 days and lateral hypothalamic electrical self-stimulation threshold increased in parallel to body weight gain (). A similar insensitivity of the reward system was previously seen in addicted rats, self-administering intravenous cocaine or heroin (, ). Furthermore, dopamine D2-receptor expression in the dorsal striatum was significantly reduced in parallel to worsening of the reward threshold (), to levels found in cocaine-addicted rats (). Interestingly, after 14 days of abstinence from the palatable diet, reward threshold did not normalize even though the rats were hypophagic and lost ∼10% body weight (). This is in contrast to the relatively rapid (∼48 h) normalization in reward thresholds in rats abstaining from cocaine self-administration () and may indicate the presence of irreversible changes caused by the high-fat content of the diet (see next section). Given the observation that cocaine addicts and obese human subjects exhibit low D2R availability in the dorsal striatum (), these findings suggest that dopamine plasticity due to repeated consumption of palatable food is somewhat similar to that due to repeated consumption of drugs of abuse.

As with drug (, , ) and alcohol (, ) addiction, abstinence from sucrose can cause craving and withdrawal symptoms (), eventually leading to relapse behavior (, ). It is thought that abstinence incubates further neural and molecular changes (, ), facilitating cue-evoked retrieval of automated behavioral programs. Therefore, relapse behavior has come under intense investigation as it is key for interrupting the addictive cycle and prevention of further spiraling dependence (). Little is known how this incubation affects “liking” and “wanting” of palatable food and how it interacts with obesity, and the schematic diagram in Fig. 3 is an attempt to outline the major pathways and processes.

Fig. 3. 

Conceptual representation of mechanisms in palatable food-induced hyperphagia. An environment of plenty favors habitual intake of palatable foods that can accelerate to an addiction-like state when normal reward processing is corrupted by hyperactivity

In summary, early observations in rodents suggest that some palatable foods such as sucrose have addictive potential in certain experimental animal models, as they recapitulate at least some key criteria established for drugs and alcohol. However, much further research is necessary to obtain a clearer picture of the abuse potential of certain foods and the neural pathways involved.

Is the obese state changing reward mechanisms and accelerating the process?

Obesity is associated with dysregulated signaling systems, such as leptin and insulin resistance, as well as increased signaling through proinflammatory cytokines and pathways activated by oxidative and endoplasmatic reticulum stress (). It is becoming clear that the obesity-induced toxic internal environment does not spare the brain (, , , , , , , , , , ). Obesity-induced brain insulin resistance is believed to have a direct effect on development of Alzheimer’s disease now also called type 3 diabetes (, ) as well as other neurodegenerative diseases ().

A number of recent studies directed attention to the hypothalamus, where high-fat diets disturb the delicate relationship between glial cells and neurons through increased endoplasmic reticulum and oxidative stress, leading to stress-response pathways with generally cytotoxic effects (, , , ). The end effects of these changes are central insulin and leptin resistance and impaired hypothalamic regulation of energy balance, further favoring the development of obesity and in turn, neurodegeneration. However, these toxic effects do not stop at the level of the hypothalamus, but can also affect brain areas involved in reward processing. The obese, leptin-deficient mouse is much more sensitive to chemically-induced neurodegeneration such as metamphetamine-induced dopamine nerve terminal degeneration as indicated by reduced striatal dopamine levels (). Obesity and hypertriglyceridemia produce cognitive impairment in mice, including reduced lever pressing for food reward (), and epidemiological studies show an association of body mass index and risk of Parkinson disease and cognitive decline (). Obesity-prone rats allowed to become obese on regular chow, or fed amounts of high-fat diet so as not to gain extra body weight, exhibited significantly reduced operant responding (progressive ratio break point) for sucrose, amphetamine-induced conditioned place preference, and dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens (). These results suggest that both obesity per se and high-fat diet can cause alterations in mesolimbic dopamine signaling and reward behaviors. Possible pathways and mechanisms by which dietary manipulations and obesity could affect neural reward circuitry are shown in Fig. 4.

Fig. 4. 

Secondary effects of obesity on reward circuitry and hypothalamic energy balance regulation. Palatable and high-fat diets can lead to obesity with or without hyperphagia. Increased inflammatory, mitochondrial, and oxidative stress signaling within the

In summary, it seems clear that the obesity-induced internal toxic environment does not stop at the level of the brain, and within the brain does not stop at the reward circuitry. Just like brain areas involved in homeostatic energy balance regulation, such as the hypothalamus, and in cognitive control, such as the hippocampus and neocortex, reward circuitry in corticolimbic and other areas is likely to be affected by obesity-induced changes in peripheral signals to the brain and local brain signaling through inflammatory, oxidative, and mitochondrial stress pathways.

Conclusions and Perspectives

Obesity clearly is a multifactorial disease with a number of potential causes, but the involvement of recent environmental changes including overabundance of palatable food and little opportunity to work off the extra energy seems undeniable. Given these external conditions together with the strong inherent bias of the homeostatic regulatory system to defend against energy depletion more strongly than energy surplus, weight is easily gained but not so easily lost. This review examines the evidence for individual differences in brain reward mechanisms as being responsible for either becoming obese or staying lean in the modern environment. Although there is considerable indirect and correlative evidence for involvement of the reward system in causing obesity in both animals and humans, there is no smoking gun for a single specific neural pathway or molecule. This is most likely because the reward system is complex and cannot be easily manipulated with drugs or genetic deletions. The most convincing evidence exists for a role of the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in the “wanting” aspect of ingestive behavior, but it is not yet clear whether over- or under-activity of dopamine signaling is at the origin of hyperphagia. Furthermore, it is not yet clear whether mesolimbic dopamine projections to selective targets in the basal ganglia, cortex, or hypothalamus are specifically involved. However, the final decision to ingest a food item, whether it is the result of conscious reasoning or subconscious emotional processing, is perhaps the most important neural process. Besides instant gratification, it takes into account the achievement of a deeper happiness that comes from living a healthy, harmonious, and successful life. For example, some individuals derive pleasure and happiness from physical activity and its long-term effects. Yet, we do not understand how the brain computes this longer-term reward and how it is integrated with the more instant pleasures.

GRANTS

This work was supported by National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases Grants DK-47348 and DK-071082.

DISCLOSURES

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the author(s).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Laurel Patterson and Katie Bailey for help with editing and Christopher Morrison and Heike Muenzberg for many discussions.

REFERENCES

1. Abizaid A, Liu ZW, Andrews ZB, Shanabrough M, Borok E, Elsworth JD, Roth RH, Sleeman MW, Picciotto MR, Tschop MH, Gao XB, Horvath TL. Ghrelin modulates the activity and synaptic input organization of midbrain dopamine neurons while promoting appetite. J Clin Invest 116: 3229–3239, 2006 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
2. Adamantidis A, de Lecea L. The hypocretins as sensors for metabolism and arousal. J Physiol 587: 33–40, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
3. Ahima RS, Qi Y, Singhal NS, Jackson MB, Scherer PE. Brain adipocytokine action and metabolic regulation. Diabetes 55, Suppl 2: S145–S154, 2006 [PubMed]
4. Ahmed SH, Kenny PJ, Koob GF, Markou A. Neurobiological evidence for hedonic allostasis associated with escalating cocaine use. Nat Neurosci 5: 625–626, 2002 [PubMed]
5. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Evidence for sugar addiction: behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32: 20–39, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
6. Baird JP, Choe A, Loveland JL, Beck J, Mahoney CE, Lord JS, Grigg LA. Orexin-A hyperphagia: hindbrain participation in consummatory feeding responses. Endocrinology 150: 1202–1216, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
7. Balcita-Pedicino JJ, Sesack SR. Orexin axons in the rat ventral tegmental area synapse infrequently onto dopamine and gamma-aminobutyric acid neurons. J Comp Neurol 503: 668–684, 2007 [PubMed]
8. Bello NT, Lucas LR, Hajnal A. Repeated sucrose access influences dopamine D2 receptor density in the striatum. Neuroreport 13: 1575–1578, 2002 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
9. Bello NT, Sweigart KL, Lakoski JM, Norgren R, Hajnal A. Restricted feeding with scheduled sucrose access results in an upregulation of the rat dopamine transporter. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 284: R1260–R1268, 2003 [PubMed]
10. Berridge KC. Food reward: brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 20: 1–25, 1996 [PubMed]
11. Berridge KC. Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24: 173–198, 2000 [PubMed]
12. Berridge KC, Ho CY, Richard JM, Difeliceantonio AG. The tempted brain eats: pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating disorders. Brain Res 1350: 43–64, 2010 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
13. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 199: 457–480, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
14. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci 26: 507–513, 2003 [PubMed]
15. Berthoud HR. Multiple neural systems controlling food intake and body weight. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26: 393–428, 2002 [PubMed]
16. Berthoud HR, Baettig K. Effects of insulin and 2-deoxy-d-glucose on plasma glucose level and lateral hypothalamic eating threshold in the rat. Physiol Behav 12: 547–556, 1974 [PubMed]
17. Berthoud HR, Baettig K. Effects of nutritive and nonnutritive stomach loads on plasma glucose level and lateral hypothalamic eating threshold in the rat. Physiol Behav 12: 1015–1019, 1974 [PubMed]
18. Bienkowski P, Rogowski A, Korkosz A, Mierzejewski P, Radwanska K, Kaczmarek L, Bogucka-Bonikowska A, Kostowski W. Time-dependent changes in alcohol-seeking behaviour during abstinence. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 14: 355–360, 2004 [PubMed]
19. Blum K, Braverman ER, Holder JM, Lubar JF, Monastra VJ, Miller D, Lubar JO, Chen TJ, Comings DE. Reward deficiency syndrome: a biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. J Psychoactive Drugs 32, Suppl i-iv: 1–112, 2000 [PubMed]
20. Blundell JE, Herberg LJ. Relative effects of nutritional deficit and deprivation period on rate of electrical self-stimulation of lateral hypothalamus. Nature 219: 627–628, 1968 [PubMed]
21. Boggiano MM, Chandler PC, Viana JB, Oswald KD, Maldonado CR, Wauford PK. Combined dieting and stress evoke exaggerated responses to opioids in binge-eating rats. Behav Neurosci 119: 1207–1214, 2005 [PubMed]
22. Borgland SL, Taha SA, Sarti F, Fields HL, Bonci A. Orexin A in the VTA is critical for the induction of synaptic plasticity and behavioral sensitization to cocaine. Neuron 49: 589–601, 2006 [PubMed]
23. Bouret SG, Simerly RB. Developmental programming of hypothalamic feeding circuits. Clin Genet 70: 295–301, 2006 [PubMed]
24. Bruce-Keller AJ, Keller JN, Morrison CD. Obesity and vulnerability of the CNS. Biochim Biophys Acta 1792: 395–400, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
25. Cai XJ, Evans ML, Lister CA, Leslie RA, Arch JR, Wilson S, Williams G. Hypoglycemia activates orexin neurons and selectively increases hypothalamic orexin-B levels: responses inhibited by feeding and possibly mediated by the nucleus of the solitary tract. Diabetes 50: 105–112, 2001 [PubMed]
26. Carelli RM. The nucleus accumbens and reward: neurophysiological investigations in behaving animals. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 1: 281–296, 2002 [PubMed]
27. Carlini VP, Martini AC, Schioth HB, Ruiz RD, Fiol de Cuneo M, de Barioglio SR. Decreased memory for novel object recognition in chronically food-restricted mice is reversed by acute ghrelin administration. Neuroscience 153: 929–934, 2008 [PubMed]
28. Christie MJ. Cellular neuroadaptations to chronic opioids: tolerance, withdrawal and addiction. Br J Pharmacol 154: 384–396, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
29. Cone RD, Cowley MA, Butler AA, Fan W, Marks DL, Low MJ. The arcuate nucleus as a conduit for diverse signals relevant to energy homeostasis. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 25, Suppl 5: S63–S67, 2001 [PubMed]
30. Cornier MA, Von Kaenel SS, Bessesen DH, Tregellas JR. Effects of overfeeding on the neuronal response to visual food cues. Am J Clin Nutr 86: 965–971, 2007 [PubMed]
31. Corwin RL. Bingeing rats: a model of intermittent excessive behavior? Appetite 46: 11–15, 2006 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
32. Corwin RL, Grigson PS. Symposium overview–food addiction: fact or fiction? J Nutr 139: 617–619, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
33. Cottone P, Sabino V, Steardo L, Zorrilla EP. Intermittent access to preferred food reduces the reinforcing efficacy of chow in rats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 295: R1066–R1076, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
34. Cripps RL, Archer ZA, Mercer JG, Ozanne SE. Early life programming of energy balance. Biochem Soc Trans 35: 1203–1204, 2007 [PubMed]
35. Dalley JW, Fryer TD, Brichard L, Robinson ES, Theobald DE, Laane K, Pena Y, Murphy ER, Shah Y, Probst K, Abakumova I, Aigbirhio FI, Richards HK, Hong Y, Baron JC, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science 315: 1267–1270, 2007 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
36. Davidowa H, Heidel E, Plagemann A. Differential involvement of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors and inhibition by dopamine of hypothalamic VMN neurons in early postnatally overfed juvenile rats. Nutr Neurosci 5: 27–36, 2002 [PubMed]
37. Davidowa H, Li Y, Plagemann A. Altered responses to orexigenic (AGRP, MCH) and anorexigenic (α-MSH, CART) neuropeptides of paraventricular hypothalamic neurons in early postnatally overfed rats. Eur J Neurosci 18: 613–621, 2003 [PubMed]
38. Davis C, Carter JC. Compulsive overeating as an addiction disorder. A review of theory and evidence. Appetite 53: 1–8, 2009 [PubMed]
39. Davis C, Levitan RD, Kaplan AS, Carter J, Reid C, Curtis C, Patte K, Hwang R, Kennedy JL. Reward sensitivity and the D2 dopamine receptor gene: a case-control study of binge eating disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32: 620–628, 2008 [PubMed]
40. Davis CA, Levitan RD, Reid C, Carter JC, Kaplan AS, Patte KA, King N, Curtis C, Kennedy JL. Dopamine for “wanting” and opioids for “liking”: a comparison of obese adults with and without binge eating. Obesity (Silver Spring) 17: 1220–1225, 2009 [PubMed]
41. Davis JF, Tracy AL, Schurdak JD, Tschop MH, Lipton JW, Clegg DJ, Benoit SC. Exposure to elevated levels of dietary fat attenuates psychostimulant reward and mesolimbic dopamine turnover in the rat. Behav Neurosci 122: 1257–1263, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
42. Dayan P, Balleine BW. Reward, motivation, and reinforcement learning. Neuron 36: 285–298, 2002 [PubMed]
43. de Araujo IE, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hobden P. Representation of umami taste in the human brain. J Neurophysiol 90: 313–319, 2003 [PubMed]
44. de Araujo IE, Oliveira-Maia AJ, Sotnikova TD, Gainetdinov RR, Caron MG, Nicolelis MA, Simon SA. Food reward in the absence of taste receptor signaling. Neuron 57: 930–941, 2008 [PubMed]
45. de Araujo IE, Rolls ET, Kringelbach ML, McGlone F, Phillips N. Taste-olfactory convergence, and the representation of the pleasantness of flavour, in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci 18: 2059–2068, 2003 [PubMed]
46. de la Monte SM. Insulin resistance and Alzheimer’s disease. BMB Rep 42: 475–481, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
47. de la Monte SM, Wands JR. Alzheimer’s disease is type 3 diabetes-evidence reviewed. J Diabetes Sci Technol 2: 1101–1113, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
48. De Souza CT, Araujo EP, Bordin S, Ashimine R, Zollner RL, Boschero AC, Saad MJ, Velloso LA. Consumption of a fat-rich diet activates a proinflammatory response and induces insulin resistance in the hypothalamus. Endocrinology 146: 4192–4199, 2005 [PubMed]
49. Del Parigi A, Chen K, Salbe AD, Reiman EM, Tataranni PA. Are we addicted to food? Obes Res 11: 493–495, 2003 [PubMed]
50. Diano S, Farr SA, Benoit SC, McNay EC, da Silva I, Horvath B, Gaskin FS, Nonaka N, Jaeger LB, Banks WA, Morley JE, Pinto S, Sherwin RS, Xu L, Yamada KA, Sleeman MW, Tschop MH, Horvath TL. Ghrelin controls hippocampal spine synapse density and memory performance. Nat Neurosci 9: 381–388, 2006 [PubMed]
51. Diano S, Horvath B, Urbanski HF, Sotonyi P, Horvath TL. Fasting activates the nonhuman primate hypocretin (orexin) system and its postsynaptic targets. Endocrinology 144: 3774–3778, 2003 [PubMed]
52. Dugan LL, Ali SS, Shekhtman G, Roberts AJ, Lucero J, Quick KL, Behrens MM. IL-6 mediated degeneration of forebrain GABAergic interneurons and cognitive impairment in aged mice through activation of neuronal NADPH oxidase. PLoS One 4: e5518, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
53. Elias CF, Kelly JF, Lee CE, Ahima RS, Drucker DJ, Saper CB, Elmquist JK. Chemical characterization of leptin-activated neurons in the rat brain. J Comp Neurol 423: 261–281, 2000 [PubMed]
54. Elmquist JK. Hypothalamic pathways underlying the endocrine, autonomic, and behavioral effects of leptin. Physiol Behav 74: 703–708, 2001 [PubMed]
55. Enriori PJ, Evans AE, Sinnayah P, Jobst EE, Tonelli-Lemos L, Billes SK, Glavas MM, Grayson BE, Perello M, Nillni EA, Grove KL, Cowley MA. Diet-induced obesity causes severe but reversible leptin resistance in arcuate melanocortin neurons. Cell Metab 5: 181–194, 2007 [PubMed]
56. Epstein DH, Shaham Y. Cheesecake-eating rats and the question of food addiction. Nat Neurosci 13: 529–531 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
57. Farooqi IS, Bullmore E, Keogh J, Gillard J, O’Rahilly S, Fletcher PC. Leptin regulates striatal regions and human eating behavior. Science 317: 1355, 2007 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
58. Farooqi S, O’Rahilly S. Genetics of obesity in humans. Endocr Rev 27: 710–718, 2006 [PubMed]
59. Farr SA, Yamada KA, Butterfield DA, Abdul HM, Xu L, Miller NE, Banks WA, Morley JE. Obesity and hypertriglyceridemia produce cognitive impairment. Endocrinology 149: 2628–2636, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
60. Felsted JA, Ren X, Chouinard-Decorte F, Small DM. Genetically determined differences in brain response to a primary food reward. J Neurosci 30: 2428–2432 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
61. Figlewicz DP. Adiposity signals and food reward: expanding the CNS roles of insulin and leptin. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 284: R882–R892, 2003 [PubMed]
62. Frazier CR, Mason P, Zhuang X, Beeler JA. Sucrose exposure in early life alters adult motivation and weight gain. PLoS One 3: e3221, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
63. Fulton S, Pissios P, Manchon RP, Stiles L, Frank L, Pothos EN, Maratos-Flier E, Flier JS. Leptin regulation of the mesoaccumbens dopamine pathway. Neuron 51: 811–822, 2006 [PubMed]
64. Fulton S, Woodside B, Shizgal P. Modulation of brain reward circuitry by leptin. Science 287: 125–128, 2000 [PubMed]
65. Geiger BM, Behr GG, Frank LE, Caldera-Siu AD, Beinfeld MC, Kokkotou EG, Pothos EN. Evidence for defective mesolimbic dopamine exocytosis in obesity-prone rats. FASEB J 22: 2740–2746, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
66. Getchell TV, Kwong K, Saunders CP, Stromberg AJ, Getchell ML. Leptin regulates olfactory-mediated behavior in ob/ob mice. Physiol Behav 87: 848–856, 2006 [PubMed]
67. Gluckman PD, Hanson MA, Beedle AS. Early life events and their consequences for later disease: a life history and evolutionary perspective. Am J Hum Biol 19: 1–19, 2007 [PubMed]
68. Gordon MD, Scott K. Motor control in a Drosophila taste circuit. Neuron 61: 373–384, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
69. Grigson PS. Like drugs for chocolate: separate rewards modulated by common mechanisms? Physiol Behav 76: 389–395, 2002 [PubMed]
70. Grill HJ, Norgren R. The taste reactivity test. II. Mimetic responses to gustatory stimuli in chronic thalamic and chronic decerebrate rats. Brain Res 143: 281–297, 1978 [PubMed]
71. Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Shaham Y. Neuroadaptation. Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature 412: 141–142, 2001 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
72. Grimm JW, Manaois M, Osincup D, Wells B, Buse C. Naloxone attenuates incubated sucrose craving in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 194: 537–544, 2007 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
73. Grimm JW, Osincup D, Wells B, Manaois M, Fyall A, Buse C, Harkness JH. Environmental enrichment attenuates cue-induced reinstatement of sucrose seeking in rats. Behav Pharmacol 19: 777–785, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
74. Guan XM, Hess JF, Yu H, Hey PJ, van der Ploeg LH. Differential expression of mRNA for leptin receptor isoforms in the rat brain. Mol Cell Endocrinol 133: 1–7, 1997 [PubMed]
75. Hajnal A, Smith GP, Norgren R. Oral sucrose stimulation increases accumbens dopamine in the rat. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 286: R31–R37, 2004 [PubMed]
76. Hare TA, O’Doherty J, Camerer CF, Schultz W, Rangel A. Dissociating the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of goal values and prediction errors. J Neurosci 28: 5623–5630, 2008 [PubMed]
77. Harris GC, Wimmer M, Aston-Jones G. A role for lateral hypothalamic orexin neurons in reward seeking. Nature 437: 556–559, 2005 [PubMed]
78. Harvey J, Shanley LJ, O’Malley D, Irving AJ. Leptin: a potential cognitive enhancer? Biochem Soc Trans 33: 1029–1032, 2005 [PubMed]
79. Harvey J, Solovyova N, Irving A. Leptin and its role in hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Prog Lipid Res 45: 369–378, 2006 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
80. Hernandez L, Hoebel BG. Feeding and hypothalamic stimulation increase dopamine turnover in the accumbens. Physiol Behav 44: 599–606, 1988 [PubMed]
81. Hoebel BG. Feeding and self-stimulation. Ann NY Acad Sci 157: 758–778, 1969 [PubMed]
82. Hoebel BG. Inhibition and disinhibition of self-stimulation and feeding: hypothalamic control and postingestional factors. J Comp Physiol Psychol 66: 89–100, 1968 [PubMed]
83. Hoebel BG, Teitelbaum P. Hypothalamic control of feeding and self-stimulation. Science 135: 375–377, 1962 [PubMed]
84. Hommel JD, Trinko R, Sears RM, Georgescu D, Liu ZW, Gao XB, Thurmon JJ, Marinelli M, DiLeone RJ. Leptin receptor signaling in midbrain dopamine neurons regulates feeding. Neuron 51: 801–810, 2006 [PubMed]
85. Hu G, Jousilahti P, Nissinen A, Antikainen R, Kivipelto M, Tuomilehto J. Body mass index and the risk of Parkinson disease. Neurology 67: 1955–1959, 2006 [PubMed]
86. Huang XF, Koutcherov I, Lin S, Wang HQ, Storlien L. Localization of leptin receptor mRNA expression in mouse brain. Neuroreport 7: 2635–2638, 1996 [PubMed]
87. Hyman SE, Malenka RC, Nestler EJ. Neural mechanisms of addiction: the role of reward-related learning and memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 29: 565–598, 2006 [PubMed]
88. Jerlhag E, Egecioglu E, Dickson SL, Douhan A, Svensson L, Engel JA. Ghrelin administration into tegmental areas stimulates locomotor activity and increases extracellular concentration of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens. Addict Biol 12: 6–16, 2007 [PubMed]
89. Johnson PM, Kenny PJ. Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nat Neurosci 13: 635–641, 2010 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
90. Julliard AK, Chaput MA, Apelbaum A, Aime P, Mahfouz M, Duchamp-Viret P. Changes in rat olfactory detection performance induced by orexin and leptin mimicking fasting and satiation. Behav Brain Res 183: 123–129, 2007 [PubMed]
91. Kaczmarek HJ, Kiefer SW. Microinjections of dopaminergic agents in the nucleus accumbens affect ethanol consumption but not palatability. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 66: 307–312, 2000 [PubMed]
92. Keen-Rhinehart E, Bartness TJ. Peripheral ghrelin injections stimulate food intake, foraging, and food hoarding in Siberian hamsters. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 288: R716–R722, 2005 [PubMed]
93. Kelley AE, Baldo BA, Pratt WE. A proposed hypothalamic-thalamic-striatal axis for the integration of energy balance, arousal, and food reward. J Comp Neurol 493: 72–85, 2005 [PubMed]
94. Kelley AE, Berridge KC. The neuroscience of natural rewards: relevance to addictive drugs. J Neurosci 22: 3306–3311, 2002 [PubMed]
95. Kleinridders A, Schenten D, Konner AC, Belgardt BF, Mauer J, Okamura T, Wunderlich FT, Medzhitov R, Bruning JC. MyD88 signaling in the CNS is required for development of fatty acid-induced leptin resistance and diet-induced obesity. Cell Metab 10: 249–259, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
96. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu Rev Psychol 59: 29–53, 2008 [PubMed]
97. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the ‘dark side’ of drug addiction. Nat Neurosci 8: 1442–1444, 2005 [PubMed]
98. Korotkova TM, Sergeeva OA, Eriksson KS, Haas HL, Brown RE. Excitation of ventral tegmental area dopaminergic and nondopaminergic neurons by orexins/hypocretins. J Neurosci 23: 7–11, 2003 [PubMed]
99. Kringelbach ML. Food for thought: hedonic experience beyond homeostasis in the human brain. Neuroscience 126: 807–819, 2004 [PubMed]
100. Kringelbach ML, O’Doherty J, Rolls ET, Andrews C. Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb Cortex 13: 1064–1071, 2003 [PubMed]
101. Leinninger GM, Jo YH, Leshan RL, Louis GW, Yang H, Barrera JG, Wilson H, Opland DM, Faouzi MA, Gong Y, Jones JC, Rhodes CJ, Chua S, Jr, Diano S, Horvath TL, Seeley RJ, Becker JB, Munzberg H, Myers MG., Jr Leptin acts via leptin receptor-expressing lateral hypothalamic neurons to modulate the mesolimbic dopamine system and suppress feeding. Cell Metab 10: 89–98, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
102. Leinninger GM, Myers MG., Jr LRb signals act within a distributed network of leptin-responsive neurones to mediate leptin action. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 192: 49–59, 2008 [PubMed]
103. Lenard NR, Berthoud HR. Central and peripheral regulation of food intake and physical activity: pathways and genes. Obesity (Silver Spring) 16, Suppl 3: S11–S22, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
104. Levine AS, Kotz CM, Gosnell BA. Sugars: hedonic aspects, neuroregulation, and energy balance. Am J Clin Nutr 78: 834S–842S, 2003 [PubMed]
105. Li XL, Aou S, Oomura Y, Hori N, Fukunaga K, Hori T. Impairment of long-term potentiation and spatial memory in leptin receptor-deficient rodents. Neuroscience 113: 607–615, 2002 [PubMed]
106. Liang NC, Hajnal A, Norgren R. Sham feeding corn oil increases accumbens dopamine in the rat. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 291: R1236–R1239, 2006 [PubMed]
107. Louis GW, Leinninger GM, Rhodes CJ, Myers MG., Jr Direct innervation and modulation of orexin neurons by lateral hypothalamic LepRb neurons. J Neurosci 30: 11278–11287, 2010 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
108. Lowe MR, van Steenburgh J, Ochner C, Coletta M. Neural correlates of individual differences related to appetite. Physiol Behav 97: 561–571, 2009 [PubMed]
109. Malik S, McGlone F, Bedrossian D, Dagher A. Ghrelin modulates brain activity in areas that control appetitive behavior. Cell Metab 7: 400–409, 2008 [PubMed]
110. Markou A, Koob GF. Postcocaine anhedonia. An animal model of cocaine withdrawal. Neuropsychopharmacology 4: 17–26, 1991 [PubMed]
111. McClure SM, Berns GS, Montague PR. Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron 38: 339–346, 2003 [PubMed]
112. Mercer JG, Moar KM, Hoggard N. Localization of leptin receptor (Ob-R) messenger ribonucleic acid in the rodent hindbrain. Endocrinology 139: 29–34, 1998 [PubMed]
113. Mogenson GJ, Jones DL, Yim CY. From motivation to action: functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Prog Neurobiol 14: 69–97, 1980 [PubMed]
114. Mueller KL, Hoon MA, Erlenbach I, Chandrashekar J, Zuker CS, Ryba NJ. The receptors and coding logic for bitter taste. Nature 434: 225–229, 2005 [PubMed]
115. Nakamura T, Uramura K, Nambu T, Yada T, Goto K, Yanagisawa M, Sakurai T. Orexin-induced hyperlocomotion and stereotypy are mediated by the dopaminergic system. Brain Res 873: 181–187, 2000 [PubMed]
116. Naleid AM, Grace MK, Cummings DE, Levine AS. Ghrelin induces feeding in the mesolimbic reward pathway between the ventral tegmental area and the nucleus accumbens. Peptides 26: 2274–2279, 2005 [PubMed]
117. O’Doherty J, Rolls ET, Francis S, Bowtell R, McGlone F, Kobal G, Renner B, Ahne G. Sensory-specific satiety-related olfactory activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroreport 11: 893–897, 2000 [PubMed]
118. O’Doherty JP, Buchanan TW, Seymour B, Dolan RJ. Predictive neural coding of reward preference involves dissociable responses in human ventral midbrain and ventral striatum. Neuron 49: 157–166, 2006 [PubMed]
119. Opland DM, Leinninger GM, Myers MG., Jr Modulation of the mesolimbic dopamine system by leptin. Brain Res 1350: 65–70, 2010 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
120. Orlet Fisher J, Rolls BJ, Birch LL. Children’s bite size and intake of an entree are greater with large portions than with age-appropriate or self-selected portions. Am J Clin Nutr 77: 1164–1170, 2003 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
121. Ozcan L, Ergin AS, Lu A, Chung J, Sarkar S, Nie D, Myers MG, Jr, Ozcan U. Endoplasmic reticulum stress plays a central role in development of leptin resistance. Cell Metab 9: 35–51, 2009 [PubMed]
122. Pecina S, Berridge KC. Hedonic hot spot in nucleus accumbens shell: where do μ-opioids cause increased hedonic impact of sweetness? J Neurosci 25: 11777–11786, 2005 [PubMed]
123. Pelchat ML. Of human bondage: food craving, obsession, compulsion, and addiction. Physiol Behav 76: 347–352, 2002 [PubMed]
124. Perry ML, Leinninger GM, Chen R, Luderman KD, Yang H, Gnegy ME, Myers MG, Jr, Kennedy RT. Leptin promotes dopamine transporter and tyrosine hydroxylase activity in the nucleus accumbens of Sprague-Dawley rats. J Neurochem 114: 666–674, 2010 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
125. Peyron C, Tighe DK, van den Pol AN, de Lecea L, Heller HC, Sutcliffe JG, Kilduff TS. Neurons containing hypocretin (orexin) project to multiple neuronal systems. J Neurosci 18: 9996–10015, 1998 [PubMed]
126. Plagemann A. Perinatal programming and functional teratogenesis: impact on body weight regulation and obesity. Physiol Behav 86: 661–668, 2005 [PubMed]
127. Posey KA, Clegg DJ, Printz RL, Byun J, Morton GJ, Vivekanandan-Giri A, Pennathur S, Baskin DG, Heinecke JW, Woods SC, Schwartz MW, Niswender KD. Hypothalamic proinflammatory lipid accumulation, inflammation, and insulin resistance in rats fed a high-fat diet. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 296: E1003–E1012, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
128. Pothos EN, Sulzer D, Hoebel BG. Plasticity of quantal size in ventral midbrain dopamine neurons: possible implications for the neurochemistry of feeding and reward (Abstract). Appetite 31: 405, 1998 [PubMed]
129. Rankinen T, Zuberi A, Chagnon YC, Weisnagel SJ, Argyropoulos G, Walts B, Perusse L, Bouchard C. The human obesity gene map: the 2005 update. Obesity (Silver Spring) 14: 529–644, 2006 [PubMed]
130. Ravussin E, Bogardus C. Energy balance and weight regulation: genetics versus environment. Br J Nutr 83, Suppl 1: S17–S20, 2000 [PubMed]
131. Ren X, Zhou L, Terwilliger R, Newton SS, de Araujo IE. Sweet taste signaling functions as a hypothalamic glucose sensor. Front Integr Neurosci 3: 1–15, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
132. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: an incentive-sensitization view. Addiction 95, Suppl 2: S91–S117, 2000 [PubMed]
133. Rogers PJ, Smit HJ. Food craving and food “addiction”: a critical review of the evidence from a biopsychosocial perspective. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 66: 3–14, 2000 [PubMed]
134. Rolls BJ, Morris EL, Roe LS. Portion size of food affects energy intake in normal-weight and overweight men and women. Am J Clin Nutr 76: 1207–1213, 2002 [PubMed]
135. Rolls BJ, Rolls ET, Rowe EA, Sweeney K. Sensory specific satiety in man. Physiol Behav 27: 137–142, 1981 [PubMed]
136. Rolls ET. The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cereb Cortex 10: 284–294, 2000 [PubMed]
137. Rolls ET, Critchley HD, Browning A, Hernadi I. The neurophysiology of taste and olfaction in primates, and umami flavor. Ann NY Acad Sci 855: 426–437, 1998 [PubMed]
138. Rolls ET, Critchley HD, Browning AS, Hernadi I, Lenard L. Responses to the sensory properties of fat of neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci 19: 1532–1540, 1999 [PubMed]
139. Rolls ET, McCabe C, Redoute J. Expected value, reward outcome, and temporal difference error representations in a probabilistic decision task. Cereb Cortex 18: 652–663, 2008 [PubMed]
140. Rolls ET, Sienkiewicz ZJ, Yaxley S. Hunger modulates the responses to gustatory stimuli of single neurons in the caudolateral orbitofrontal cortex of the macaque monkey. Eur J Neurosci 1: 53–60, 1989 [PubMed]
141. Rolls ET, Verhagen JV, Kadohisa M. Representations of the texture of food in the primate orbitofrontal cortex: neurons responding to viscosity, grittiness, and capsaicin. J Neurophysiol 90: 3711–3724, 2003 [PubMed]
142. Rosenbaum M, Sy M, Pavlovich K, Leibel RL, Hirsch J. Leptin reverses weight loss-induced changes in regional neural activity responses to visual food stimuli. J Clin Invest 118: 2583–2591, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
143. Rothwell NJ, Stock MJ. Luxuskonsumption, diet-induced thermogenesis and brown fat: the case in favour. Clin Sci (Colch) 64: 19–23, 1983 [PubMed]
144. Rozengurt E, Sternini C. Taste receptor signaling in the mammalian gut. Curr Opin Pharmacol 7: 557–562, 2007 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
145. Russo SJ, Dietz DM, Dumitriu D, Morrison JH, Malenka RC, Nestler EJ. The addicted synapse: mechanisms of synaptic and structural plasticity in nucleus accumbens. Trends Neurosci 33: 267–276, 2010 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
146. Saper CB, Chou TC, Elmquist JK. The need to feed: homeostatic and hedonic control of eating. Neuron 36: 199–211, 2002 [PubMed]
147. Schmid DA, Held K, Ising M, Uhr M, Weikel JC, Steiger A. Ghrelin stimulates appetite, imagination of food, GH, ACTH, and cortisol, but does not affect leptin in normal controls. Neuropsychopharmacology 30: 1187–1192, 2005 [PubMed]
148. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275: 1593–1599, 1997 [PubMed]
149. Schwartz MW. Brain pathways controlling food intake and body weight. Exp Biol Med (Maywood) 226: 978–981, 2001 [PubMed]
150. Schwartz MW, Woods SC, Porte D, Jr, Seeley RJ, Baskin DG. Central nervous system control of food intake. Nature 404: 661–671, 2000 [PubMed]
151. Sclafani A. Carbohydrate-induced hyperphagia and obesity in the rat: effects of saccharide type, form, and taste. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 11: 155–162, 1987 [PubMed]
152. Sclafani A. Carbohydrate taste, appetite, obesity: an overview. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 11: 131–153, 1987 [PubMed]
153. Sclafani A, Ackroff K. The relationship between food reward and satiation revisited. Physiol Behav 82: 89–95, 2004 [PubMed]
154. Sclafani A, Springer D. Dietary obesity in adult rats: similarities to hypothalamic and human obesity syndromes. Physiol Behav 17: 461–471, 1976 [PubMed]
155. Seckl JR. Physiologic programming of the fetus. Clin Perinatol 25: 939–962, vii, 1998 [PubMed]
156. See RE. Neural substrates of conditioned-cued relapse to drug-seeking behavior. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 71: 517–529, 2002 [PubMed]
157. Shigemura N, Ohta R, Kusakabe Y, Miura H, Hino A, Koyano K, Nakashima K, Ninomiya Y. Leptin modulates behavioral responses to sweet substances by influencing peripheral taste structures. Endocrinology 145: 839–847, 2004 [PubMed]
158. Shin AC, Pistell PJ, Phifer CB, Berthoud HR. Reversible suppression of food reward behavior by chronic μ-opioid receptor antagonism in the nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience 170: 580–588, 2010 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
159. Shioda S, Funahashi H, Nakajo S, Yada T, Maruta O, Nakai Y. Immunohistochemical localization of leptin receptor in the rat brain. Neurosci Lett 243: 41–44, 1998 [PubMed]
160. Silva JP, von Meyenn F, Howell J, Thorens B, Wolfrum C, Stoffel M. Regulation of adaptive behaviour during fasting by hypothalamic Foxa2. Nature 462: 646–650, 2009 [PubMed]
161. Skaper SD. The brain as a target for inflammatory processes and neuroprotective strategies. Ann NY Acad Sci 1122: 23–34, 2007 [PubMed]
162. Small DM. Individual differences in the neurophysiology of reward and the obesity epidemic. Int J Obes (Lond) 33, Suppl 2: S44–S48, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
163. Small DM, Jones-Gotman M, Zatorre RJ, Petrides M, Evans AC. A role for the right anterior temporal lobe in taste quality recognition. J Neurosci 17: 5136–5142, 1997 [PubMed]
164. Small DM, Zald DH, Jones-Gotman M, Zatorre RJ, Pardo JV, Frey S, Petrides M. Human cortical gustatory areas: a review of functional neuroimaging data. Neuroreport 10: 7–14, 1999 [PubMed]
165. Smith GP. Accumbens dopamine mediates the rewarding effect of orosensory stimulation by sucrose. Appetite 43: 11–13, 2004 [PubMed]
166. Smith GP. The direct and indirect controls of meal size. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 20: 41–46, 1996 [PubMed]
167. Sorensen LB, Moller P, Flint A, Martens M, Raben A. Effect of sensory perception of foods on appetite and food intake: a review of studies on humans. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 27: 1152–1166, 2003 [PubMed]
168. Speakman JR. A nonadaptive scenario explaining the genetic predisposition to obesity: the “predation release” hypothesis. Cell Metab 6: 5–12, 2007 [PubMed]
169. Speakman JR. Thrifty genes for obesity, an attractive but flawed idea, and an alternative perspective: the ‘drifty gene’ hypothesis. Int J Obes (Lond) 32: 1611–1617, 2008 [PubMed]
170. Sriram K, Benkovic SA, Miller DB, O’Callaghan JP. Obesity exacerbates chemically induced neurodegeneration. Neuroscience 115: 1335–1346, 2002 [PubMed]
171. Steiner JE. The Gustofacial Response: Observations on Normal and Anancephalic Newborn Infants. Bethesda, MD: U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1973, p. 125–167
172. Stice E, Spoor S, Bohon C, Small DM. Relation between obesity and blunted striatal response to food is moderated by TaqIA A1 allele. Science 322: 449–452, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
173. Stice E, Spoor S, Bohon C, Veldhuizen MG, Small DM. Relation of reward from food intake and anticipated food intake to obesity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Abnorm Psychol 117: 924–935, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
174. Stice E, Spoor S, Ng J, Zald DH. Relation of obesity to consummatory and anticipatory food reward. Physiol Behav 97: 551–560, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
175. Stratford TR, Kelley AE. Evidence of a functional relationship between the nucleus accumbens shell and lateral hypothalamus subserving the control of feeding behavior. J Neurosci 19: 11040–11048, 1999 [PubMed]
176. Teegarden SL, Scott AN, Bale TL. Early life exposure to a high fat diet promotes long-term changes in dietary preferences and central reward signaling. Neuroscience 162: 924–932, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
177. Thaler JP, Choi SJ, Schwartz MW, Wisse BE. Hypothalamic inflammation and energy homeostasis: resolving the paradox. Front Neuroendocrinol 31: 79–84, 2010 [PubMed]
178. Tordoff MG. Obesity by choice: the powerful influence of nutrient availability on nutrient intake. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 282: R1536–R1539, 2002 [PubMed]
179. Travers SP, Norgren R. Organization of orosensory responses in the nucleus of the solitary tract of rat. J Neurophysiol 73: 2144–2162, 1995 [PubMed]
180. Treit D, Spetch ML, Deutsch JA. Variety in the flavor of food enhances eating in the rat: a controlled demonstration. Physiol Behav 30: 207–211, 1983 [PubMed]
181. Uher R, Treasure J, Heining M, Brammer MJ, Campbell IC. Cerebral processing of food-related stimuli: effects of fasting and gender. Behav Brain Res 169: 111–119, 2006 [PubMed]
182. Unger EK, Piper ML, Olofsson LE, Xu AW. Functional role of c-Jun-N-terminal kinase in feeding regulation. Endocrinology 151: 671–682 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
183. Valenstein ES, Cox VC, Kakolewski JW. Hypothalamic motivational systems: fixed or plastic neural circuits? Science 163: 1084, 1969 [PubMed]
184. Velkoska E, Cole TJ, Dean RG, Burrell LM, Morris MJ. Early undernutrition leads to long-lasting reductions in body weight and adiposity whereas increased intake increases cardiac fibrosis in male rats. J Nutr 138: 1622–1627, 2008 [PubMed]
185. Vengeliene V, Bilbao A, Molander A, Spanagel R. Neuropharmacology of alcohol addiction. Br J Pharmacol 154: 299–315, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
186. Verhagen JV. The neurocognitive bases of human multimodal food perception: consciousness. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2006
187. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Telang F. Overlapping neuronal circuits in addiction and obesity: evidence of systems pathology. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 363: 3191–3200, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
188. Volkow ND, Wise RA. How can drug addiction help us understand obesity? Nat Neurosci 8: 555–560, 2005 [PubMed]
189. Vuilleumier P, Driver J. Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: windows on causal interactions between human brain regions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 362: 837–855, 2007 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
190. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Thanos PK, Fowler JS. Similarity between obesity and drug addiction as assessed by neurofunctional imaging: a concept review. J Addict Dis 23: 39–53, 2004 [PubMed]
191. Wansink B, Kim J. Bad popcorn in big buckets: portion size can influence intake as much as taste. J Nutr Educ Behav 37: 242–245, 2005 [PubMed]
192. Wansink B, van Ittersum K, Painter JE. Ice cream illusions bowls, spoons, and self-served portion sizes. Am J Prev Med 31: 240–243, 2006 [PubMed]
193. Warwick ZS, Schiffman SS. Role of dietary fat in calorie intake and weight gain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 16: 585–596, 1992 [PubMed]
194. Wise RA. Forebrain substrates of reward and motivation. J Comp Neurol 493: 115–121, 2005 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
195. Woods SC, D’Alessio DA, Tso P, Rushing PA, Clegg DJ, Benoit SC, Gotoh K, Liu M, Seeley RJ. Consumption of a high-fat diet alters the homeostatic regulation of energy balance. Physiol Behav 83: 573–578, 2004 [PubMed]
196. Wyvell CL, Berridge KC. Intra-accumbens amphetamine increases the conditioned incentive salience of sucrose reward: enhancement of reward “wanting” without enhanced “liking” or response reinforcement. J Neurosci 20: 8122–8130, 2000 [PubMed]
197. Yarmolinsky DA, Zuker CS, Ryba NJ. Common sense about taste: from mammals to insects. Cell 139: 234–244, 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
198. Zhang X, Zhang G, Zhang H, Karin M, Bai H, Cai D. Hypothalamic IKKβ/NF-κB and ER stress link overnutrition to energy imbalance and obesity. Cell 135: 61–73, 2008 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
199. Zheng H, Patterson LM, Berthoud HR. Orexin signaling in the ventral tegmental area is required for high-fat appetite induced by opioid stimulation of the nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci 27: 11075–11082, 2007 [PubMed]