The Relationship Between Sexual Content on Mass Media and Social Media: A Longitudinal Study (2015)

Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2015 Nov 20.

Vandenbosch L1, van Oosten JM1, Peter J1.

Abstract

The goal of this study was to investigate whether exposure to sexual reality television content and Internet pornography (IP) is related to sexual self-presentation on social media.

Based on a two-wave panel survey among 1,765 adolescents aged 13-17 years, we found that watching sexual reality television content stimulated adolescents to produce and distribute sexual images of themselves on social media. In turn, sexual self-presentation on social media led adolescents to watch sexual reality television content more frequently. These relationships were similar among boys and girls.

No reciprocal relationship between exposure to IP and boys’ and girls’ sexual self-presentation on social media was found. The results suggest that sexual content in mainstream mass media may predict adolescents’ sexually oriented behavior on social media and vice versa.

Moreover, adolescents seem to differentiate between types of sexual content (i.e., mainstream versus more explicit sexual content) when incorporating sexual media content in their sexual behavior online.

Author information

Laura Vandenbosch, PhD, Johanna M.F. van Oosten, PhD, and Jochen Peter, PhD

The Amsterdam School of Communication Research, ASCoR, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands.

Address correspondence to:

Dr. Johanna M.F. van Oosten

The Amsterdam School of Communication Research, ASCoR

University of Amsterdam

P.O. Box 15791

1001 NG Amsterdam

Netherlands
E-mail: [email protected]

Introduction

Social media are highly popular among adolescents, with adolescents checking news feeds and post updates daily.1 Recently, research has shown that adolescents also use social media to distribute sexually suggestive images of themselves.2–4 For example, a content analysis revealed that one in five adolescents showed sexually revealing images on his or her online profile.5 Another study demonstrated that up to half of the teenage profiles contain a sexy image of the adolescent user.6 However, although research on the prevalence of sexual self-presentations on social media has accumulated,5–7 little is known on why adolescents choose to present themselves in a sexual way on their online profiles.

Against this background, scholars have observed that the prevalence of sexual self-presentations on social media seems to reflect the prevalence of sexual messages in mass media content popular with adolescents.7–9 Consequently, scholars studying mass10 as well as social8 media have called for research, studying relationships between exposure to sexual content in mass media and the use of social media to distribute user-generated sexual content. To address this lacuna, the current study aims to investigate associations between exposure to sexual content in mass media and boys’ and girls’ sexual self-presentations on social media.

Regarding mass media, the study will focus on sexually oriented reality television content and Internet pornography (IP) because of their popularity among adolescents and their high degree of sexual content. Reality television attracts large numbers of adolescent audiences11,12 and is characterized by its focus on sex.11,13–16 In terms of IP, most individuals are likely to encounter pornography in adolescence17,18 with approximately 10 percent identifying themselves as frequent users.19 IP can be described as “professionally produced or user-generated pictures or videos (clips) on or from the Internet that are intended to arouse the viewer. These videos and pictures depict sexual activities, such as masturbation as well as oral, anal, and vaginal penetration, in an unconcealed way, often with a close-up on genitals.”19(pp1015–1016) Content analyses have shown that both reality television and IP regularly portray ideal bodies and emphasize the sexual appeal of the characters.20–24

Due to the importance of sexual attractiveness in mass media, frequent consumers of these media may be more inclined to present themselves also in a sexual way. Social cognitive theory25 posits that exposure to environmental incentives (e.g., observing the sexual behavior of attractive models in media content) may stimulate individuals to behave accordingly (e.g., engage in sexual behavior that is similar to the behavior of the observed models). Accordingly, research has shown that sexual television viewing relates to a younger age of dating initiation26 and a greater number of dating partners.26 Studies have also found that using IP is positively associated with more sexual partners27–29 and a greater variety of sexual activities.30 However, we still lack knowledge on the relationship between exposure to sexual messages in mass media and the extent to which users present themselves in a sexual way on social media. As prior research suggests that young users’ behavior is related to the sexual behavior of models in mass media, we hypothesize that exposure to sexual reality television content (H1) and IP (H2) will positively predict a sexual self-presentation on social media.

Next to the relationship between mass media exposure and user-generated content on social media, an inverse process also seems conceivable. Cognitive dissonance theory, for instance, posits that individuals are motivated to search for information that is cognitively consonant with their own cognitions and behaviors.31 Individuals may thus avoid the unease that emerges when encountering cognitively dissonant information.31 In line with this, longitudinal research has shown that being sexually active stimulated the selection of sexual content in television, music, magazines, and video games over time.32 Accordingly, if adolescents present themselves in a sexual way on social media, they may prefer consuming mass media content, in which the characters also present themselves as sexy. Therefore, we hypothesize that a sexual self-presentation on social media will increase exposure to sexual reality television content (H3) and IP (H4). Hypotheses 1–4 are summarized in Figure 1.

http://online.liebertpub.com/na101/home/literatum/publisher/mal/journals/content/cyber/0/cyber.ahead-of-print/cyber.2015.0197/20151119/images/small/figure1.gif View larger version (33K)

FIG. 1.  The hypothesized model for the relationships between exposure to sexual content in mass media (i.e., sexual reality television content and Internet pornography) and a sexual self-presentation on social media.

Information about Downloading

When studying the reciprocal relationship between exposure to sexual content in mass media and a sexual online self-presentation, it is important to consider potential gender differences. Gender socialization theory highlights that girls and boys are socialized toward different but complementary sexual attitudes and behaviors.33 While boys are expected to play an active role in sexual relationships, girls are encouraged to adopt a rather passive role.33 In this context, sexual attractiveness is more strongly valued for girls than for boys,33 which in turn may be related to girls presenting themselves more frequently in a sexual way on social media.5,34–36

Differences between boys and girls have also been found in how media exposure relates to adolescents’ sexual behavior. In line with the active role of boys, a recent longitudinal study37 found that sexual media exposure stimulated sexual behavior only among boys. Conversely, sexual behavior triggered sexual media exposure only among girls. The study thus suggested that a media effect occurred among boys, while a selection effect occurred among girls. Possibly, sexual media exposure encourages boys to search actively for a sexual relationship, while girls seek validation of their sexual behavior in their media use (as it is less consistent with their passive sexual role).37 However, other studies38–40 that examined relationships between exposure to mass sexual media and sexual outcomes have not found gender differences. Against this background, we ask whether gender moderates the reciprocal relationships between exposure to sexual reality television content/IP and a sexual self-presentation on social media (RQ1).

Top of Form

Bottom of Form

Methods

Procedure

The current study draws on the first two waves of a three-wave panel study with an interval of 6 months. The first two waves were conducted in May and October 2013. We selected the first two waves because two popular reality shows were broadcast during that time (see descriptions of exposure to sexual reality television content) in Netherlands. The study was carried out among 13- to 17-year-old adolescents. Sampling and fieldwork were done and organized by Veldkamp, a Dutch survey institute. The sample was randomly sampled from an existing nationally representative online access panel of adolescents, administered by Veldkamp. Participants filled in an online questionnaire at home, which took about 20 minutes to complete. For each completed questionnaire, participants received a compensation of 5 Euros.

Sample

At baseline, 2,137 adolescents participated. Six months later, 1,765 adolescents participated again (attrition rate = 17.4 percent). Using Pillai’s Trace, a MANOVA showed that there were no significant differences between respondents participating only in Wave 1 and respondents participating in both waves regarding age, sexual orientation, gender, exposure to sexual reality television content, exposure to IP, and a sexual online self-presentation, V = 0.005, F(6, 2130) = 1.73, p = 0.11, ηp2 = 0.005. It is thus unlikely that attrition caused a systematic bias in the data.

Measures

Descriptive statistics and psychometric properties for all relevant variables and scales are shown in Table 1.

Data table

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics and Zero-Order Correlations (N = 1,765)

Demographical information

Respondents indicated their age and gender (0 = boy; 1 = girl). Sexual orientation was measured by the H-scale41 and recoded according to the procedure applied by Peter and Valkenburg19 (0 = exclusively heterosexual; 1 = not exclusively heterosexual).

Exposure to sexual reality television content

With a seven-point Likert scale (1 = never to 7 = every episode), we measured how often respondents watched two reality shows (a) MTV’s “Jersey Shore” and (b) MTV’s “Geordie Shore” during the 6 months before the survey. These sexually oriented reality shows were broadcast before and during data collection.

Exposure to IP

Respondents indicated the extent to which they had intentionally watched, on the Internet, (a) pictures with clearly exposed genitals, (b) videos with clearly exposed genitals, (c) pictures in which people are having sex, (d) or videos in which people are having sex, on a seven-point scale (never = 1 through several times a day = 7).42 Principal component analysis suggested that all items loaded on one factor (Time 1 eigenvalue = 3.56; explained variance = 88.96 percent).

Sexual online self-presentation

If respondents used social media, they were asked to indicate, for the past 6 months and on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = never to 7 = always), how often they had uploaded pictures portraying themselves (a) with a sexy gaze, (b) with a sexy appearance, (c) scantily dressed (e.g., bathing suit or underwear), and (d) in a sexy posture. Adolescents who had never used social media at Waves 1 and/or 2 (n = 179)a were given the code 1 (“never”), as they never have had the possibility to present themselves in a sexual way. Principal component analysis suggested all items loaded on one factor (Time 1 eigenvalue = 2.81; explained variance = 70.13 percent).

Analytical strategy

Structural equation modeling (software AMOS 7), maximum likelihood estimation method, was used to test the hypotheses and the model in Figure 1. Each latent variable was predicted by the manifest items used to measure that construct: exposure to sexual reality television content was predicted by two manifest items; exposure to IP and sexual online self-presentation were each predicted by four manifest items (see Measures section). Consistent with prior sexual media research,42 baseline values of age and sexual orientation were entered as control variables and were expected to predict endogenous variables. Moreover, the control variables and the independent variables at baseline were allowed to covary with each other. Similarly, the disturbance terms of the media variables at Time 2 and the error terms of the identical items were modeled to covary between Time 1 and Time 2.

As the normality assumption is often violated in sexuality research,19 bootstrapping (95 percent bias-corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals; 1,000 samples) was used to validate the significance tests based on normal test theory. Finally, to examine gender differences, the fit indices of an unconstrained model were compared with the fit indices of a constrained model (in which either the reciprocal relationship between a sexual self-presentation on social media and exposure to (1) sexual reality television content or (2) IP was constrained to be equal among boys and girls). The χ2-model comparison test value and ΔCFI were used to test for gender differences.43,44

Top of Form

Bottom of Form

Results

The model had an acceptable fit of the data (for zero-order correlations, see Table 1; for goodness-of-fit statistics, see Table 2). Watching sexual reality television at Time 1 positively predicted a sexual self-presentation on social media at Time 2 (for effect parameters, see Table 2). Moreover, a sexual online self-presentation at Time 1 was positively associated with watching sexual reality television at Time 2, thus supporting H1 and H3. Watching IP at Time 1 did not predict a sexual self-presentation on social media at Time 2. In addition, a sexual self-presentation on social media at Time 1 was unrelated to watching IP at Time 2. H2 and H4 were not supported.

Data table

Table 2. Structural Equation Modeling Results for Key Paths (N = 1,765)

The model comparison tests for sexual reality television content and IP (Table 2; RQ1) indicated that the χ2-difference test was not significant and that the differences between the CFI values (ΔCFI) of both the unconstrained and the constrained models did not exceed 0.01. The model fit of the unconstrained model was thus neither superior to the model constraining the reciprocal relationship between sexual reality television content and a sexual self-presentation on social media nor to the model constraining the reciprocal relationship between IP and a sexual self-presentation on social media to be equal across gender. As no gender differences emerged, the path results of the unconstrained model are not presented in Table 2.

Top of Form

Bottom of Form

Discussion

This study is one of the first to study the relationship between exposure to sexual messages in mass media and adolescents’ tendency to present themselves in a sexual way on social media. The study points to the importance of sexual messages in mainstream mass media content in motivating adolescents’ sexual self-presentation online. While exposure to sexual messages in sexual reality television content was reciprocally related to a sexual self-presentation on social media, no reciprocal relationship was found when studying exposure to IP. The study has several important implications for future research.

First, the reciprocal relationship between exposure to sexual reality television content and a sexual self-presentation on social media among boys and girls highlights the potential of mainstream entertainment on television to affect how adolescents behave in their online environment. The finding also suggests that adolescents who adopt a sexual self-presentation on social media may seek out, in particular, mainstream sexual media content on television. More generally, the reciprocal pattern between sexual reality television content and a sexual self-presentation on social media points to cyclical processes, as specified in theories, such as the Media Practice Model45 and the reinforcing spirals model.46 In such cyclical processes, adolescents’ sexual self-presentation online and their exposure to sexual content in mainstream media influence and strengthen each other. Reality TV may be particularly relevant in this respect given that adolescents often look for people or situations in the media that are “credible” and “like them.”45,47 However, the literature has also indicated that adolescents identify with characters from other popular television genres.48 As popular genres, such as music videos and soap operas, also frequently portray sexual characters,24,49 future research may explore whether similar cyclical processes between watching these genres and a sexual online self-presentation can be found.

Second, media theories, such as the Differential Susceptibility to Media Effects Model, have highlighted that (most) media effects may not hold equally for the whole (adolescent) population.50 Particular dispositional susceptibility factors (described as person dimensions that affect a user’s interaction with media content) may strengthen or weaken media effects among the general population of media users.50 The current findings suggest that gender is not an important dispositional susceptibility variable for the reciprocal relationships between a sexual self-presentation on social media and exposure to sexual reality television content or IP. However, other dispositional susceptibility variables may still affect these relationships. Although IP exposure and a sexual self-presentation on social media were unrelated in the current study, this relationship may thus still occur among groups of users who are more susceptible to the effects of IP or more likely to select IP. In this view, the pertinent literature points to high sensation seekers,51 hypergendered adolescents,52 and adolescents in an early pubertal status40 as important groups to examine.

That said, it is possible that exposure to IP and a sexual self-presentation on social media are unrelated because they differ in their sexual explicitness. A sexual self-presentation on social media5,7 is typically only sexually suggestive, while IP is sexually explicit. Adolescents may perceive the actors and actresses in IP as inappropriate exemplars. In line with this reasoning, qualitative research has shown that girls make sure that their online self-presentations are not considered “slutty.”53 Similarly, a sexual self-presentation on social media may not be considered as similar to the sexually explicit content in IP. Adolescents who present themselves in a sexual way on social media may thus not be motivated to consume IP.

Our study had at least two limitations: first, our study applied self-report measures of adolescents’ sexual self-presentations. This measure only taps whether adolescents present themselves in sexual ways on social media, but provides limited information on how adolescents present themselves. To understand how adolescents incorporate sexual messages from mainstream media in their online self-presentations, we need more detailed measures of sexual self-presentation, including both visual and verbal posts.

Second, the effect sizes of the reciprocal relationship between exposure to sexual reality television and a sexual self-presentation on social media were small, although in line with prior media research54 and literature on longitudinal research controlling for stability effects.55 Moreover, these relatively small effect sizes may be explained by the rather low occurrence of a sexy self-presentation among the adolescents included in our sample. Despite this low frequency score, a relationship between exposure to sexual reality television and an online sexual self-presentation still emerged, which highlights the importance of future research on this subject. In addition, the literature56 suggests that even small effects of media can still be of relevance as the sexual messages promoted in the studied media content (i.e., reality television and social media) are similar to the socialization received from other sources (e.g., other mainstream sexual media content and peers2,53). Together, these socialization influences may cumulate over time in a stronger effect.56

Top of Form

Bottom of Form

Conclusion

Overall, the current study shows that mainstream mass media content has the potential to stimulate adolescents to produce and distribute their own sexual self-portrayals. In turn, the sexual content in mainstream mass media appears to be particularly appealing to social media users who present themselves in a sexual way. Future research among adolescents is therefore warranted to deepen our knowledge about the interplay between mainstream sexual content in mass media and sexually oriented behaviors on social media.

Top of Form

Bottom of Form

Note

a. All the structural equation models reported in the results section were also conducted with a sample that excluded the participants who never used social networking site (SNS) at Time 1 and/or Time 2 (N = 1,586). The structural equation modeling results were similar to the results reported in the article for the sample that included participants who never used SNS at Time 1 and/or Time 2 (N = 1,765). These additional results can be obtained by sending an e-mail to the corresponding author.

Top of Form

Bottom of Form

Acknowledgment

This research was funded by a grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) to the third author.

Top of Form

Bottom of Form

Author Disclosure Statement

No competing financial interests exist.

Top of Form

Bottom of Form

References

1. A Lenhart, K Purcell, A Smith, et al. (2010) Social media and mobile internet use among teens and young adults. Washington, DC: Pew Internet American Life Project.

2. SM Doornwaard, MA Moreno, RJJM van den Eijnden, et al. Young adolescents’ sexual and romantic reference displays on Facebook. Journal of Adolescent Health 2014; 55:535–541.

3. RM Perloff. Social media effects on young women’s body image concerns: theoretical perspectives and an agenda for research. Sex Roles 2014; 71:363–377.

4. JMF Van Oosten, J Peter, I Boot. Exploring associations between exposure to sexy online self-presentations and adolescents’ sexual attitudes and behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 2015; 44:1078–1091.

5. S Kapidzic, SC Herring. Race, gender, and self-presentation in teen profile photographs. New Media and Society 2015; 17:958–976.

6. L Crescenzi, N Arauna, I Tortajada. Privacy, self-disclosure and self-image of Spanish teenagers on social networking sites. The case of Fotolog. Communication and Society 2013; 26:65–78.

7. PC Hall, JH West, E McIntyre. Female self-sexualization in MySpace.com personal profile photographs. Sexuality and Culture 2012; 16:1–16.

8. M Prieler, J Choi. Broadening the scope of social media effect research on body image concerns. Sex Roles 2014; 71:378–388.

9. A Hirdman. (2007) “‘Please vote nicely … .’” Visualizing gender on-line. In S Knudsen, L Lofgren-Martenson, S Mansson, eds. Generation P? Youth, gender and pornography. Copenhagen: Danish School of Education Press, pp. 151–170.

10. EW Owens, RJ Behun, JC Manning, et al. The impact of Internet pornography on adolescents: a review of the research. Sexual Addiction and Compulsivity 2012; 19:99–122.

11. BJ Bond, KL Drogos. Sex on the shore: wishful identification and parasocial relationships as mediators in the relationship between Jersey Shore exposure and emerging adults’ sexual attitudes and behaviors. Media Psychology 2014; 17:102–126.

12. K Barton. Reality television programming and diverging gratifications: the influence of content on gratifications obtained. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media 2009; 53:460–476.

13. LM Ward, L Reed, SL Trinh, et al. (2014) Sexuality and entertainment media. In DL Tolman, LM Diamond, JA Bauermeister, et al., eds. APA Handbook of sexuality and psychology. Volume 2. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 373–427.

14. K Farrar, D Kunkel, E Biely, et al. Sexual messages during prime-time programming. Sexuality and Culture 2003; 7:7–37.

15. D Kunkel, K Eyal, E Donnerstein, et al. Sexual socialization messages on entertainment television: comparing content trends 1997–2002. Media Psychology 2007; 9:595–622.

16. SL Smith. From Dr. Dre to dismissed: assessing violence, sex, and substance use on MTV. Critical Studies in Media Communication 2005; 22:89–98.

17. A Ševčíková, K Daneback. Online pornography use in adolescence: age and gender differences. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 2014; 11:674–686.

18. M Weber, O Quiring, G Daschmann. Peers, parents and pornography: exploring adolescents’ exposure to sexually explicit material and its developmental correlates. Sexuality and Culture 2012; 16:408–427.

19. J Peter, PM Valkenburg. The use of sexually explicit internet material and its antecedents: a longitudinal comparison of adolescents and adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior 2011; 40:1015–1025.

20. DR Arakawa, C Flanders, E Hatfield. Are variations in gender equality evident in pornography? A cross-cultural study. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 2012; 36:279–285.

21. M Barron, M Kimmel. Sexual violence in three pornographic media: toward a sociological explanation. Journal of Sex Research 2000; 37:161–168.

22. M Klaassen, J Peter. Gender (in)equality in Internet pornography: a content analysis of popular pornographic Internet videos. Journal of Sex Research 2015; 52:721–735.

23. SA Vannier, AB Currie, LF O’Sullivan. Schoolgirls and soccer moms: a content analysis of free “teen” and “MILF” online pornography. Jounal of Sex Research 2014; 51:253–264.

24. L Vandenbosch, D Vervloessem, S Eggermont. “I might get your heart racing in my skin-tight jeans”: sexualization on music entertainment television. Communication Studies 2013; 64:178–194.

25. A Bandura. Social cognitive theory of mass communication. Media Psychology 2001; 3:265–299.

26. R Rivadeneyra, MJ Lebo. The association between television-viewing behaviors and adolescent dating role attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Adolescence 2008; 31:291–305.

27. SC Boies. University students uses of and reactions to online sexual information and entertainment: links to online and offline sexual behavior. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 2002; 11:77–89.

28. DK Braun-Courville, M Rojas. Exposure to sexually explicit websites and adolescent sexual attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health 2009; 45:156–162.

29. EM Morgan. Associations between young adults’ use of sexually explicit materials and their sexual preferences, behaviors, and satisfaction. Journal of Sex Research 2011; 48:520–530.

30. A Stulhofer, V Busko, I Landripet. Pornography, sexual socialization, and satisfaction among young men. Archives of Sexual Behavior 2010; 39:168–178.

31. L Festinger. A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations 1954; 7:117–140.

32. A Bleakley, M Hennessy, M Fishbein, et al. It works both ways: the relationship between exposure to sexual content in the media and adolescent sexual behavior. Media Psychology 2008; 11:443–461.

33. DL Tolman, M Striepe, T Harmon. Gender matters: constructing a model of adolescent sexual health. Journal of Sex Research 2003; 40:4–12.

34. J Bailey, V Steeves, J Burkell, et al. Negotiating with gender stereotypes on social networking sites: from “bicycle face” to facebook. Journal of Communication Inquiry 2013; 37:91–112.

35. AM Manago, MB Graham, PM Greenfield, et al. Self-presentation and gender on MySpace. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 2008; 29:446–458.

36. S Thiel-Stern. Femininity out of control on the Internet: a critical analysis of media representations of gender, youth, and MySpace.com in international news discourses. Girlhood Studies 2009; 2:20–39.

37. E Frison, L Vandenbosch, J Trekels, et al. Reciprocal relationships between music television exposure and adolescents’ sexual behaviors: the role of perceived peer norms. Sex Roles 2015; 72:183–197.

38. J Peter, PM Valkenburg. Adolescents’ exposure to sexually explicit Internet material and sexual satisfaction: a longitudinal study. Human Communication Research 2009; 35:171–194.

39. L Vandenbosch, S Eggermont. The role of mass media in adolescents’ sexual behaviors: exploring the explanatory value of the three-step self-objectification process. Archives of Sexual Behavior 2014; 44:729–742.

40. L Vandenbosch, S Eggermont. Sexually explicit websites and sexual initiation: reciprocal relationships and the moderating role of pubertal status. Journal of Research on Adolescence 2012; 23:621–634.

41. AC Kinsey, WB Pomeroy, CE Martin. (1948) Sexual behavior in the human male. Philadelphia, PA: Saunder.

42. J Peter, PM Valkenburg. Adolescents’ exposure to sexually explicit Internet material and sexual preoccupancy: a three-wave panel study. Media Psychology 2008; 11:207–234.

43. BM Byrne. (2010) Structural equation modeling with AMOS: basic concepts, applications and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

44. GW Cheung, RB Rensvold. Evaluating goodness-of- fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 2002; 9:233–255.

45. JR Steele, JD Brown. Adolescent room culture: studying media in the context of everyday life. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 1995; 24:551–576.

46. MD Slater. Reinforcing spirals: the mutual influence of media selectivity and media effects and their impact on individual behavior and social identity. Communication Theory 2007; 17:281–301.

47. JR Steele. Teenage sexuality and media practice: factoring in the influences of family, friends, and school. Journal of Sex Research 1999; 36:331–341.

48. LM Ward, R Rivadeneyra. Contributions of entertainment television to adolescents’ sexual attitudes and expectations: the role of viewing amount versus viewer involvement. Journal of Sex Research 1999; 36:237–249.

49. LM Ward. Talking about sex: common themes about sexuality in the prime-time television programs children and adolescents view most. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 1995; 24:595–615.

50. PM Valkenburg, J Peter. The differential susceptibility to media effects model. Journal of Communication 2013; 63:221–243.

51. L Vandenbosch, I Beyens. Sexually oriented television viewing and adolescents’ attitude toward uncommitted sexual exploration in Belgium: the moderating role of sensation seeking and gender. Journal of Children and Media 2014; 8:183–200.

52. JMF Van Oosten, J Peter, I Boot. Women’s critical responses to sexually explicit material: the role of hyperfemininity and processing style. Journal of Sex Research 2015; 52:306–316.

53. J Ringrose. (2009) Sluts, whores, fat slags and playboy bunnies: teen girls’ negotiations of “sexy” on social networking sites and at school. In C Jackson, C Paechter, E Renold, eds. Girls and education 3–16: continuing concerns, new agendas. New York, NY: McGraw Hill Open University Press, pp. 170–182.

54. PM Valkenburg, J Peter. Five challenges for the future of media-effects research. International Journal of Communication 2013; 7:197–215.

55. P Adachi, T Willoughby. Interpreting effect sizes when controlling for stability effects in longitudinal autoregressive models: implications for psychological science. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 2014; 12:116–128.

56. G Gerbner, L Gross, M Morgan, et al. (1986) Living with television: the dynamics of the cultivation process. In J Bryant, D Zillmann, eds. Perspectives on media effects. Hillsdale, NJ: Erblaum, pp. 17–40.