Neuropharmacology. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as:
- Neuropharmacology. 2009; 56(Suppl 1): 160–168.
- Published online 2008 Jul 9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.070
PMCID: PMC2635339
NIHMSID: NIHMS86826
Paul Vezinaa,* and Marco Leytonb
The publisher’s final edited version of this article is available at Neuropharmacology
See other articles in PMC that cite the published article.
Abstract
Repeated intermittent exposure to psychostimulants can lead to long-lasting sensitization of the drugs’ behavioral and biochemical effects. Such findings have figured importantly in recent theories of drug addiction proposing that sensitized nucleus accumbens (NAcc) dopamine (DA) overflow in particular acts in concert with other alterations in the neurochemistry of this nucleus to promote drug seeking and self-administration. Yet, experiments in rodents, nonhuman primates and humans have not always detected behavioral or biochemical sensitization following drug exposure, bringing into doubt the utility of this model. In an effort to reconcile apparent discrepancies in the literature, this review assesses conditions that might affect the expression of sensitization during testing. Specifically, the role played by conditioned cues is reviewed. A number of reports strongly support a potent and critical role for conditioned stimuli in the expression of sensitization. Findings suggest that stimuli associated either with the presence or absence of drug can respectively facilitate or inhibit sensitized responding. It is concluded that the presence or absence of such stimuli during testing for sensitization in animal and human studies could significantly affect the results obtained. It is necessary to consider this possibility especially when interpreting the results of studies that fail to observe sensitized responding.
1. Sensitization in animals and humans
There is general agreement that rats repeatedly exposed to psychostimulants like amphetamine will exhibit enhanced – sensitized – locomotor responding when subsequently challenged with the drug some time later. In these animals, the reactivity of mesoaccumbens dopamine (DA) neurons to the drug challenge is also enhanced (for critical reviews of the preclinical literature, see Kalivas and Stewart, 1991; Vanderschuren and Kalivas, 2000; Vezina, 2004). These are long-lasting effects in the rat. Sensitized locomotor responding has been reported up to one year (Paulson et al., 1991) and enhanced nucleus accumbens (NAcc) DA overflow up to three months following drug exposure (Hamamura et al., 1991). Notably, the magnitude of amphetamine-induced DA overflow in the NAcc increases with time following exposure to the drug (Vezina, 2007).
Given the importance of the mesocorticolimbic DA pathways in the generation of appetitive behaviors including the seeking and consumption of abused drugs, it follows that long-lasting enhancements in the reactivity of these pathways could lead to long-lasting enhancements in appetitive behavioral output. This possibility has figured importantly in the formulation of an influential theoretical view of addiction proposing that sensitized NAcc DA overflow acts in concert with other alterations in the neurochemistry of this nucleus to enhance the appetitive effects of drugs and promote their pursuit and self-administration (Robinson and Berridge, 1993). Consequently, a large number of systems, cellular and molecular biological level investigations have focused on the mechanisms that might underlie altered reactivity in midbrain DA neurons and those systems they interact with (Hyman et al., 2006).
It remains, however, that most of the experimental support for enhanced DA overflow stems from experiments conducted in rodents, while findings obtained in nonhuman primates and humans have been equivocal. For example, functional neuroimaging studies suggest a profile in limbic regions of reduced rather than augmented drug-induced DA responses in cocaine addicted patients compared to controls (e.g., Volkow et al., 1997). This has led to arguments that sensitization of NAcc DA responsivity as a mechanism for drug abuse and other forms of pathology is of limited value as it does not extend to the human condition. Recent evidence has emerged, however, demonstrating that amphetamine-induced DA release from ventral striatum can in fact be sensitized in human subjects (Boileau et al., 2006).
Below we first review the evidence for behavioral and dopaminergic sensitization in animals and humans as it relates to drug seeking and drug taking. Evidence that the expression of sensitization can be regulated by conditioned cues is then reviewed. The review of the animal literature is restricted to reports of experiments conducted in rats as these have provided the majority of the preclinical evidence in these areas. In an effort to reconcile apparent discrepancies in the literature, we specifically explore the possibility that the expression of sensitization can be facilitated in some cases and inhibited in others. It is argued that such effects must be considered when interpreting the results of studies that fail to observe sensitized responding.
1.1. Sensitization in animals
Amphetamine increases extracellular levels of DA in the terminal and cell body regions of mesoaccumbens DA neurons by reversing DA transport and preventing its uptake via the DA transporter (Seiden et al., 1993). In the NAcc, this effect has been associated with its ability to produce locomotor activity and support self-administration (Hoebel et al., 1983; Vezina and Stewart, 1990). Both effects are blocked by DA receptor antagonists or 6-OHDA lesions of DA nerve terminals in the NAcc (Joyce and Koob, 1981; Lyness et al., 1979; Phillips et al., 1994; Vezina, 1996).
In rats previously exposed to repeated intermittent psychostimulant injections, these effects are enhanced (see Box 1). Long-lasting sensitized locomotor and NAcc DA responding have been reported (Kalivas and Stewart, 1991; Vanderschuren and Kalivas, 2000; Vezina, 2004). In the latter case, the enhanced ability of drugs like amphetamine to increase extracellular levels of DA in the NAcc represents the neuroadaptation most consistently associated with the expression of behavioral sensitization. It increases with time, and has been observed in vitro and in vivo weeks to months following drug exposure (Hamamura et al., 1991; Kolta et al., 1989; Paulson and Robinson, 1995; Robinson, 1988, 1991; Segal and Kuczenski, 1992; Wolf et al., 1994; Vezina, 2007; cf, Kuczenski et al., 1997). The induction of sensitization by psychostimulants, on the other hand, has been found to occur in the ventral tegmental area (VTA), site of the cell bodies of mesoaccumbens DA neurons. Both locomotor and NAcc DA sensitization are produced by amphetamine in the VTA in a D1 DA receptor dependent manner (Bjijou et al., 1996; Cador et al., 1995; Dougherty and Ellinwood, 1981; Hooks et al., 1992; Kalivas and Weber, 1988; Perugini and Vezina, 1994; Vezina, 1993, 1996; Vezina and Stewart, 1990). It is likely that both types of sensitization are produced by a cascade of neuronal events initiated by increases in extracellular levels of DA in the VTA (Kalivas and Duffy, 1991). These certainly involve glutamate-DA interactions as activation of all three glutamate receptor subtypes (NMDA, AMPA and metabotropic) is required for the induction of locomotor sensitization by amphetamine (Vanderschuren and Kalivas, 2000; Vezina and Suto, 2003; Wolf, 1998).
There is also convincing evidence that repeated exposure to psychostimulants leads to their enhanced self-administration. As a limbic-motor interface (Mogenson, 1987) receiving rich sensory encoding projections from the VTA and forebrain regions like the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and basolateral amygdala, the NAcc is well positioned to play a central role in the generation of adaptive motor responses to behaviorally relevant stimuli. Because activity in mesoaccumbens DA neurons is linked not only to the locomotion produced but also to the self-administration supported by drugs like amphetamine, it is reasonable to expect that sensitized reactivity in these neurons will affect drug seeking and drug self-administration. It has been argued that activity in mesoaccumbens DA neurons encodes the incentive valence of a drug effect (Robinson and Berridge, 1993; Stewart et al., 1984; Vezina et al., 1999). If this were the case, sensitization in these neurons should similarly enhance the incentive to pursue the drug and those stimuli associated with it. Many reports supporting this view have now established that previous exposure to a number of drugs leads to enhanced conditioned place preference (Gaiardi et al., 1991; Lett, 1989; Shippenberg and Heidbreder, 1995) as well as facilitated acquisition of drug self-administration (Horger et al., 1990, 1992; Piazza et al., 1989, 1991; Pierre and Vezina, 1997; Valadez and Schenk, 1994) and, once the behavior is acquired, enhanced motivation to obtain the drug (Lorrain et al., 2000; Mendrek et al., 1998; Vezina et al., 2002). As observed with sensitized locomotion and NAcc DA overflow, the development of these effects on drug self-administration also requires the activation of D1 DA and glutamatergic receptors in the VTA (Pierre and Vezina, 1998; Suto et al., 2002, 2003).
1.2. Sensitization in humans
In the last 10-15 years, functional neuroimaging techniques have been developed such as positron emission tomography (PET) that use radioactively labeled benzamide ligands for D2/3 DA receptors and can be coupled to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). These have permitted studies of the effects of abused drugs on DA reactivity in human forebrain that recently have achieved sufficient spatial resolution to allow assessment of different striatal subregions. As established in rodents, these studies indicate that extracellular levels of DA are also increased in human striatum (especially ventral subregions) following the acute administration of various abused drugs including amphetamine (Volkow et al., 1994, 1997, 1999, 2001; Laruelle et al., 1995; Breier et al., 1997; Drevets et al., 2001; Leyton et al., 2002; Martinez et al., 2003, 2007; Abi-Dargham et al., 2003; Oswald et al., 2005; Riccardi et al., 2006a; Boileau et al., 2006, 2007; Munro et al., 2006; Casey et al., 2007) and cocaine (Schlaepfer et al., 1997; Cox et al., 2006). These drug-induced increases in extracellular DA were found to correlate with positive mood states and craving as well as novelty and sensation seeking.
While human studies are understandably more complex than those conducted in rodents, evidence that sensitization can occur to the behavioral effects of drugs has been reported, although not without some apparent inconsistencies (for a critical review of the human literature, see Leyton, 2007). When sufficiently high amphetamine concentrations were administered to non-drug dependent subjects (see Box 2), sensitization to a number of drug effects was observed, including potentiated indices of vigor and energy levels as well as potentiated eye-blink and mood-elevating responses (Strakowski et al., 1996, 2001; Strakowski and Sax, 1998; Boileau et al., 2006). In one study (Sax and Strakowski, 1998), sensitized drug-induced elevation in mood correlated positively with the personality trait of novelty seeking. In the longest study, augmented amphetamine-induced increases in vigor were observed a full year later (Boileau et al., 2006). Sensitization to how much the subjects liked the amphetamine was not typically observed in these studies, a finding consistent with evidence suggesting that NAcc DA is linked more to the motivational salience of drugs and the cues they are associated with than to the pleasure derived from their consumption (Stewart et al., 1984; Stewart, 1992; Blackburn et al., 1992; Robinson and Berridge, 1993; Berridge and Robinson, 1998; Ikemoto and Panksepp, 1999; Leyton, 2008). Interestingly, tolerance to the euphoric effects of psychostimulant drugs has been reported in cocaine dependent abusers despite enhanced drug seeking (Volkow et al., 1997; Mendelson et al., 1998). These individuals have also been reported to fail to show sensitized subjective or physiological responses following 2-4 daily cocaine administrations (Nagoshi et al., 1992; Rothman et al., 1994; Gorelick and Rothman, 1997).
Studies assessing sensitization of the striatal DA effects of psychostimulants are considerably smaller in number but their findings are somewhat consistent with the behavioral results reviewed above. When conducted in non-drug abusing subjects, significantly greater amphetamine-induced ventral striatal DA release was observed two weeks and again one year following the administration of three drug doses over a one week period (Boileau et al., 2006). The extent of DA sensitization correlated positively with sensitization of energy level and eye-blink rate as well as the personality trait of novelty seeking. However, when conducted in detoxified patients with a history of cocaine dependence, less rather than more striatal DA release was observed in response to a psychostimulant challenge (Volkow et al., 1997; Martinez et al., 2007). Importantly, this reduced DA response could not be explained as a failure of the DA system to respond as these individuals are capable of exhibiting drug cue-induced increases in DA release (reported selectively in the dorsal striatum; Volkow et al., 2006; Wong et al., 2006).
A number of significant differences exist between studies conducted in healthy subjects and drug abusing patients that might account for the different results reported. In the latter case, subjects have been exposed to substantial amounts of drug and it is possible that even in detoxified patients the intensity of this exposure may interfere with the subsequent expression of sensitization. In the rat, enhanced drug-induced NAcc DA overflow is not observed in the days following exposure but rather weeks to months later (Hamamura et al., 1991; Hurd et al., 1989; Segal and Kuczenski, 1992; Paulson and Robinson, 1995). The withdrawal period necessary to observe sensitization may be longer in humans and longer still following prolonged intense drug exposure (see Dalia et al., 1998; Vezina et al., 2007). Another critical difference between studies conducted in healthy and drug abusing subjects may involve the various environmental stimuli surrounding drug taking and those constituting the testing conditions. Drug-paired and drug-unpaired cues may differentially influence drug-induced DA responsivity in these two groups. The constellation of stimuli afforded by the PET testing environment, for example, would be expected to exert different effects in individuals that have received drug only in their presence, compared to others that have associated these cues with the absence of drug. The evidence supporting this possibility is outlined below.
2. Conditioned cues and the expression of sensitization
It has been known for some time that the expression of behavioral sensitization can come under strong conditioned environmental stimulus control. The primary evidence for this comes from experiments showing that rats previously exposed to the drug in one environment (Paired) show a greater locomotor response to the drug on a test for sensitization conducted in this environment compared to rats previously exposed to the drug elsewhere (Unpaired) or Control rats previously exposed to saline in both environments. Indeed, under these conditions, Unpaired rats fail to show any evidence for locomotor sensitization when tested with the drug, even though they have previously received the same pharmacological exposure to the drug as Paired rats. Such environment-specific expression of locomotor sensitization has been reported with different drugs including morphine, amphetamine and cocaine (Vezina and Stewart, 1984; Stewart and Vezina, 1987; Vezina et al., 1989; Pert et al., 1990; Stewart and Vezina, 1991; Anagonstaras and Robinson, 1996; Anagnostaras et al., 2002; Wang and Hsiao, 2003; Mattson et al., 2008; for reviews, see Stewart and Vezina, 1988; Stewart, 1992). Recently, this approach was used to demonstrate environment-specific sensitization of amphetamine-induced NAcc DA overflow as well (Guillory et al., 2006).
In these experiments, a discrimination procedure is often used to simultaneously expose animals to a drug and allow for associations to form between the drug unconditioned stimulus (UCS) and the environment conditioned stimulus (CS) complex (Figure 1). In locomotor activity experiments, rats in a Paired group receive drug in activity monitoring chambers on one day and saline in another environment (often the home cage) the next day. Rats in an Unpaired group receive the same number of drug injections but in the other environment and saline in the activity chambers; these rats are thus exposed to drug but unpaired with the activity chambers. Finally, a third group of Control animals is exposed equally to both environments but never to drug. This procedure allows for the subsequent testing of conditioned responding when rats in all groups are administered saline before the test and sensitized responding when all rats are administered a challenge drug injection before testing (Figure 1). Inevitably, enhanced responding is observed in Paired animals on both of these tests: conditioned locomotion on the test for conditioning and environment-specific sensitization on the test for sensitization (Figure 2).
2.1. Excitatory Pavlovian conditioning and the expression of sensitization
Not surprisingly, early attempts to account for environmental stimulus control of the expression of sensitization proposed that it was due simply to the summation of the drug UCS and the growing conditioned response to the drug-paired CS (Hinson and Poulos, 1981; Pert et al., 1990). In the rat, a number of CS-elicited conditioned responses have been demonstrated following drug-CS pairings including locomotor activity, stereotypy and rotational behavior (Beninger and Hahn, 1983; Vezina and Stewart, 1984; Carey, 1986; Drew and Glick, 1987; Hiroi and White, 1989; Pert et al., 1990; Stewart and Vezina, 1991; Anagnostaras and Robinson, 1996) as well as NAcc DA overflow (Fontana et al., 1993; Gratton and Wise, 1994; Di Ciano et al., 1998; Ito et al., 2000). Similarly, a number of CS-elicited conditioned responses have been reported in humans, including craving as well as increased euphoria, energy, drug liking, drug wanting, heart rate and systolic blood pressure (Foltin and Haney, 2000; Panlilio et al., 2005; Berger et al., 1996, Leyton et al., 2005; Boileau et al., 2007). Cue-elicited conditioned striatal DA release has also been reported in humans (Volkow et al., 2006; Wong et al., 2006; Boileau et al., 2007). However, while conditioned drug effects have been proposed to play potentially important roles in motivating drug seeking in animals and humans (Stewart et al., 1984; Childress et al., 1988; Stewart, 2004), their contribution to environment-specific sensitization is less clear. For example, the simple combination of a conditioned response and the drug UCS does not always summate to the perceived sensitized response (Anagnostaras and Robinson, 1996). In addition, some exposure regimens, such as infusing amphetamine into the VTA, do produce locomotor and NAcc DA sensitization but do not elicit an unconditioned response or lead to the development of a conditioned response, so that the expression of sensitization is context independent (Vezina and Stewart, 1990; Perugini and Vezina, 1994; Vezina, 1996; Scott-Railton et al., 2006). Similarly, in vitro striatal slice experiments showing sensitized DA release necessarily do so in the absence of contextual stimuli (Castaneda et al., 1988; Robinson and Becker, 1982), making it necessary to consider alternative explanations for how drug associated environmental cues regulate the expression of sensitization. These findings clearly show that sensitization is a non-associative phenomenon that can nonetheless come under environmental stimulus control.
2.2. Facilitation and conditioned inhibition can regulate the expression of sensitization
Anagnostaras and Robinson (1996) reported compelling findings supporting the idea that stimuli acting as facilitators (also referred to as occasion setters) can account for environment-specific sensitization. Facilitating properties are bestowed on stimuli subjected to contingencies that allow them to reliably predict the occurrence of another stimulus. Once established, these stimuli can then function as occasion setters by modulating the excitatory strength of other stimuli. Unlike conditioned excitators, facilitators do not necessarily elicit conditioned responses but rather control the ability of other stimuli to do so (Rescorla, 1985; Holland, 1992). In the case of sensitization, Anagnostaras and Robinson (1996) show that an environmental stimulus complex that comes to predict the presence of a drug can also acquire the ability to set the occasion for the sensitized response on the test day without the need to elicit an excitatory conditioned response of its own. Thus, sensitized responding was observed only in animals tested in the presence of the facilitating stimulus complex (Paired animals in Figure 1). It should be noted that the results of Anagnostaras and Robinson (1996) indicate that facilitators not only set the occasion for CSs but can also do so for drug UCSs as well (see Box 1).
Somewhat overlooked has been the additional possibility that cues specifically unpaired with the drug can come to act as conditioned inhibitors (Rescorla, 1969; LoLordo and Fairless, 1985) to prevent the expression of the sensitized response. Different lines of evidence support this possibility, proposed by Stewart and Vezina (1988, 1991; Stewart, 1992). First, the discriminative conditioning procedure outlined in Figure 1 and used in drug conditioning and sensitization studies is known to establish stimuli explicitly unpaired with the UCS as conditioned inhibitors (Mackintosh, 1974). Second, when used in a summation procedure, conditioned inhibitors reduce responding not only to conditioned excitators but to unconditioned stimuli as well (Rescorla, 1969; Thomas, 1972). Thus, as proposed by Anagnostaras and Robinson (1996) for facilitators, conditioned inhibitors could in the same way modulate responding to the unconditioned effects of a drug (Stewart, 1992). Third, procedures known to extinguish conditioned inhibition (Lysle and Fowler, 1985; Kasprow et al., 1987; Hallam et al., 1990; Fowler et al., 1991) can selectively disinhibit the expression of locomotor and NAcc DA sensitization by amphetamine to reveal sensitized responding in Unpaired animals (Guillory et al., 2006; see also Stewart and Vezina, 1991). Finally, Anagnostaras et al. (2002) showed that using electroconvulsive shock to induce retrograde amnesia disinhibited responding selectively in Unpaired rats on a test for sensitization, suggesting that these animals were normally indeed sensitized but inhibited from expressing enhanced responding. In addition to conditioned inhibition of the expression of sensitization, evidence for the conditioned inhibition of the development of tolerance to the analgesic (Siegel et al., 1981), sedative (Fanselow and German, 1982) and hypothermic (Hinson and Siegel, 1986) effects of other drugs has been reported as well.
Together, the above findings demonstrate that the expression of sensitization can come under strong environmental stimulus control. Thus, the expression of sensitized responding can be promoted by stimuli that have come to predict the presence of drug and inhibited by stimuli that have come to signal its absence. Moreover, there is no reason to suspect that such processes are mutually exclusive. Although certainly considerably more complex, such facilitating and inhibitory stimuli would also be expected to exercise strong control over the expression of sensitization in humans.
2.3. Implications for the expression of sensitization in humans
It is interesting to review some of the findings reported in human drug sensitization studies in light of the above findings. Although not the only distinction, one of the most salient differences between experiments conducted in healthy and drug abusing subjects regards the stimuli surrounding drug administration during exposure and those constituting conditions during testing. To the extent that the stimuli associated by drug abusing individuals with drug procurement and consumption most probably differ considerably from those present at the time of testing, the opportunity for either inhibition or lack of facilitation of sensitized behavioral and striatal DA responding could interfere with the expression of sensitization at test (e.g., Nagoshi et al., 1992; Rothman et al., 1994; Gorelick and Rothman, 1997; Volkow et al., 1997; Mendelson et al., 1998). Conversely, when drug naïve individuals are administered drug exclusively in the presence of testing cues, the conditions for facilitation of sensitized behavioral and DA responding could promote the expression of sensitization at test (e.g., Strakowski et al., 1996, 2001; Strakowski and Sax, 1998; Boileau et al., 2006). Consistent with this interpretation, when stimuli relevant to drug abusing subjects were made available during testing (mirror, razor blade, straw, and cocaine powder) and subjects were allowed to prepare the powder into one or two lines and to ingest it intra-nasally in their usual fashion, past psychostimulant drug use correlated positively with striatal DA response (Cox et al., 2006). Similar experiments in which these cues were not present (the drug challenge was administered non-contingently via a nondescript capsule described as a medication; no drug paraphernalia or drug-paired cues were present), past psychostimulant drug use predicted a smaller striatal DA response (Casey et al., 2007). Interestingly, a recent study reported that drug-related stimuli that – unlike those in Cox et al. (2006) – did not lead to drug taking, failed to elicit enhanced striatal DA release in drug abusing subjects (Volkow et al., 2008). These findings again confirm the importance of environmental stimuli in drug responding in that withholding of an expected reinforcer is known to diminish DA responding (Schultz et al., 1997).
3. Conclusions
An accumulating animal literature indicates that the expression of sensitization is susceptible to a wider range of factors than is usually considered. Particularly relevant are features of the drug exposure regimen prior to testing (e.g., intensity of drug exposure and duration of withdrawal) as well as the presence or absence of drug-related cues during testing (for reviews, see Leyton, 2007; Vezina et al., 2007). In this review, evidence is presented showing that the expression of sensitization to drugs of abuse can come under strong environmental stimulus control. Stimuli that predict the availability of the drug (facilitators, occasion setters) promote sensitized responding whereas stimuli that predict its absence (conditioned inhibitors) inhibit the expression of sensitization. While initially limited to locomotor responding in rodents, these results were recently extended to include the conditioned inhibition of sensitized neurochemical responses as well.
It is argued here that similar effects occur in humans. The results of a number of experiments in humans suggest that the presence of cues predicting drug availability is associated with sensitized responding while the absence of these cues or the presence of stimuli predicting the absence of drug is associated with the absence of sensitized responding. Such cues capable of affecting the expression of sensitization may thus influence vulnerability to addiction, the waxing and waning of relapse susceptibility, and the exaggerated salience attached to drug cues. Studies that do not control for these factors might not detect sensitization even when the relevant neuroadaptations have occurred and their potential to significantly alter behavior is present.
Acknowledgements
This review was made possible by grants from the National Institutes of Health (DA09397, PV) and the Canadian Institutes for Health Research (MOP-36429 and MOP-64426, ML).
Footnotes
Publisher’s Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Abi-Dargham A, Kegeles LS, Martinez D, Innis RB, Laruelle M. Dopamine mediation of positive reinforcing effects of amphetamine in stimulant naive healthy volunteers: results from a large cohort. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;13:459–468. [PubMed]
- Anagnostaras SG, Robinson TE. Sensitization to the psychomotor stimulant effects of amphetamine: modulation by associative learning. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1996;110:1397–1414. [PubMed]
- Anagnostaras SG, Schallert T, Robinson TE. Memory processes governing amphetamine-induced psychomotor sensitization. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:703–715. [PubMed]
- Beninger RJ, Hahn BL. Pimozide blocks establishment but not expression of amphetamine-produced environment-specific conditioning. Science. 1983;220:1304–1306. [PubMed]
- Berger SP, Hall S, Mickalian JD, Reid MS, Crawford CA, Delucchi K, et al. Haloperidol antagonism of cue-elicited cocaine craving. The Lancet. 1996;347:504–508. [PubMed]
- Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Research Brain Research Reviews. 1998;28:309–369. [PubMed]
- Bindra D. How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1978;1:41–52.
- Bjijou Y, Stinus L, Le Moal M, Cador M. Evidence for selective involvement of dopamine D1 receptors in the ventral tegmental area in the behavioral sensitization induced by intra-ventral tegmental area injections of d-amphetamine. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 1996;277:1177–1187. [PubMed]
- Blackburn JR, Pfaus JG, Phillips AG. Dopamine functions in appetitive and defensive behaviours. Progress in Neurobiology. 1992;39:247–279. [PubMed]
- Boileau I, Dagher A, Leyton M, Gunn RN, Baker GB, Diksic M, Benkelfat C. Modeling sensitization to stimulants in humans: A [11C]raclopride/PET study in healthy volunteers. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:1386–1395. [PubMed]
- Boileau I, Dagher A, Leyton M, Welfeld K, Booij L, Diksic M, Benkelfat C. Conditioned dopamine release in humans: A PET [11C]raclopride study with amphetamine. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:3998–4003. [PubMed]
- Breier A, Su T-P, Saunders R, Carson RE, Kolachana BS, de Bartolomeis A, Weinberger DR, Weisenfeld N, Malhotra AK, Eckelman WC, Pickar D. Schizophrenia is associated with elevated amphetamine-induced synaptic dopamine concentrations: Evidence from a novel positron emission tomography method. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. 1997;94:2569–2574. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Cador M, Bjijou Y, Stinus L. Evidence of a complete independence of the neurobiological substrates of the induction and expression of behavioral sensitization to amphetamine. Neuroscience. 1995;65:385–395. [PubMed]
- Carey RJ. Conditioned rotational behavior in rats with unilateral 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the substantia nigra. Brain Research. 1986;365:379–382. [PubMed]
- Casey KF, Benkelfat C, Dagher A, Baker GB, Leyton M. Stimulant drug exposure and family environment predict the striatal dopamine response to d-amphetamine: A PET [11C]raclopride study. Canadian College of Neuropsychopharmacology Banff; Canada: 2007. p. 15.
- Castaneda E, Becker JB, Robinson TE. The long-term effects of repeated amphetamine treatment in vivo on amphetamine, KCl and electrical stimulation evoked striatal dopamine release in vitro. Life Sciences. 1988;42:2447–2456. [PubMed]
- Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman R, O’Brien CP. Classically conditioned responses in cocaine and opioid dependence: A role in relapse? In: Ray BA, editor. Learning factors in substance abuse. NIDA Research Monograph, NIDA; Washington, DC: 1988. pp. 25–43. [PubMed]
- Cox SML, Benkelfat C, Dagher A, Delaney JS, McKenzie SA, Kolivakis T, Casey KF, Leyton M. Cocaine self-administration in humans: A PET study of serotonin-dopamine interactions. American College of Neuropsychopharmacology Hollywood; Florida: 2006. 3 – 7 December 2006.
- Dalia AD, Norman MK, Tabet MR, Schlueter KT, Tsibulsky VL, Norman AB. Transient amelioration of the sensitization of cocaine-induced behaviors in rats by the induction of tolerance. Brain Research. 1998;797:29–34. [PubMed]
- Di Ciano P, Blaha CD, Phillips AG. Conditioned changes in dopamine oxidation currents in the nucleus accumbens of rats by stimuli paired with self-administration or yoked-administration of d-amphetamine. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;10:1121–1127. [PubMed]
- Dougherty GG, Jr., Ellinwood EH., Jr. Chronic d-amphetamine in nucleus accumbens: Lack of tolerance or reverse tolerance of locomotor activity. Life Sciences. 1981;28:2295–2298. [PubMed]
- Drevets WC, Gautier CH, Price JC, Kupfer DJ, Kinahan PE, Grace AA, Price JL, Mathis CA. Amphetamine-induced dopamine release in human ventral striatum correlates with euphoria. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;49:81–96. [PubMed]
- Drew KL, Glick SD. Classical conditioning of amphetamine-induced lateralized and nonlateralized activity in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1987;92:52–57. [PubMed]
- Eikelboom R, Stewart J. The conditioning of drug-induced physiological responses. Psychological Review. 1982;89:507–528. [PubMed]
- Fanselow MS, German C. Explicitly unpaired delivery of morphine and the test situation: Extinction and retardation of tolerance to the suppressing effects of morphine in locomotor activity. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1982;35:231–241. [PubMed]
- Foltin RW, Haney M. Conditioned effects of environmental stimuli paired with smoked cocaine in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2000;149:24–33. [PubMed]
- Fontana DJ, Post RM, Pert A. Conditioned increases in mesolimbic dopamine overflow associated with cocaine. Brain Research. 1993;629:31–39. [PubMed]
- Fowler H, Lysle DT, DeVito PL. Conditioned excitation and conditioned inhibition of fear: Asymmetrical processes as evident in extinction. In: Denny MR, editor. Fear, Avoidance and phobias: A fundamental analysis. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1991. pp. 317–362.
- Gaiardi M, Bartoletti M, Bacchi A, Gubellini C, Costa M, Babbini M. Role of repeated exposure to morphine in determining its affective properties: place and taste conditioning studies in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1991;103:183–186. [PubMed]
- Gorelick DA, Rothman RB. Stimulant sensitization in humans. Biological Psychiatry. 1997;42:230–231. [PubMed]
- Gratton A, Wise RA. Drug- and behavior-associated changes in dopamine-related electrochemical signals during intravenous cocaine self-administration. Journal of Neuroscience. 1994;14:4130–4146. [PubMed]
- Guillory AM, Suto N, You Z-B, Vezina P. Effects of conditioned inhibition on neurotransmitter overflow in the nucleus accumbens. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts. 2006;32:483.3. Manuscript in submission.
- Hallam SC, Matzel LD, Sloat J, Miller RR. Excitation and inhibition as a function of posttraining extinction of the excitatory cue used in Pavlovian inhibition training. Learning and Motivation. 1990;21:59–84.
- Hamamura T, Akiyama K, Akimoto K, Kashihara K, Okumura K, Ujike H, Otsuki S. Co-administration of either a selective D1 or D2 dopamine antagonist with methamphetamine prevents methamphetamine-induced behavioral sensitization and neurochemical change, studied by in vivo intracerebral dialysis. Brain Research. 1991;546:40–6. [PubMed]
- Hinson RE, Poulos CX. Sensitization to the behavioral effects of cocaine: Modification by Pavlovian conditioning. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1981;15:559–562. [PubMed]
- Hinson RE, Siegel S. Pavlovian inhibitory conditioning and tolerance to pentobarbital-induced hypothermia in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1986;12:363–370. [PubMed]
- Hiroi N, White NM. Conditioned stereotypy: Behavioral specification of the UCS and pharmacological investigation of the neural change. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1989;32:249–258. [PubMed]
- Hoebel BG, Monaco AP, Hernandez L, Aulisi EF, Stanley BG, Lenard L. Self-injection of amphetamine directly into the brain. Psychopharmacology. 1983;81:158–163. [PubMed]
- Holland PC. Occasion setting in Pavlovian conditioning. In: Medin DL, editor. The psychology of learning and motivation. Academic Press; San Diego, CA: 1992. pp. 69–125.
- Hooks MS, Jones GH, Liem BJ, Justice JB., Jr. Sensitization and individual differences to intraperitoneal amphetamine, cocaine or caffeine following repeated intracranial amphetamine infusions. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1992;43:815–823. [PubMed]
- Horger BA, Giles MK, Schenk S. Preexposure to amphetamine and nicotine predisposes rats to self-administer a low dose of cocaine. Psychopharmacology. 1992;107:271–276. [PubMed]
- Horger BA, Shelton K, Schenk S. Pre-exposure sensitizes rats to the rewarding effects of cocaine. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1990;37:707–711. [PubMed]
- Hurd YL, Weiss F, Koob GF, Anden NE, Ungerstedt U. Cocaine reinforcement and extracellular dopamine overflow in rat nucleus accumbens: An in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Research. 1989;498:199–203. [PubMed]
- Hyman SE, Malenka RC, Nestler EJ. Neural mechanisms of addiction: The role of reward-related learning and memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2006;29:565–598. [PubMed]
- Ikemoto S, Panksepp J. The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: a unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Research Brain Research Review. 1999;31:6–41. [PubMed]
- Ito R, Dailey JW, Howes SR, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Dissociation in conditioned dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens core and shell in response to cocaine cues and during cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:7489–7495. [PubMed]
- Johanson CE, Uhlenhuth EH. Drug preference and mood in humans: Repeated assessment of d-amphetamine. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1981;14:159–163. [PubMed]
- Joyce EM, Koob GF. Amphetamine-, scopolamine- and caffeine-induced locomotor activity following 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the mesolimbic dopamine system. Psychopharmacology. 1981;73:311–313. [PubMed]
- Kalivas PW, Duffy PA. A comparison of axonal and somatodendritic dopamine release using in vivo microdialysis. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1991;56:961–967. [PubMed]
- Kalivas PW, Stewart J. Dopamine transmission in the initiation and expression of drug-and stress-induced sensitization of motor activity. Brain Research Brain Research Reviews. 1991;16:223–44. [PubMed]
- Kalivas PW, Weber B. Amphetamine injected into the ventral mesencephalon sensitizes rats to peripheral amphetamine and cocaine. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 1988;245:1095–1102. [PubMed]
- Kasprow WJ, Schachtman TR, Miller RR. The comparator hypothesis of conditioned response generation: Manifest conditioned excitation and inhibition as a function of relative excitatory strengths of CS and conditioning context at the time of testing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1987;13:395–406. [PubMed]
- Kelly TH, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. The effects of repeated amphetamine exposure on multiple measures of human behavior. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1991;38:417–426. [PubMed]
- Kolta MG, Shreve P, Uretsky NJ. Effect of pretreatment with amphetamine on the interaction between amphetamine and dopamine neurons in the nucleus accumbens. Neuropharmacology. 1989;28:9–14. [PubMed]
- Kuczenski R, Segal D, Todd PK. Behavioral sensitization and extracellular dopamine responses to amphetamine after various treatments. Psychopharmacology. 1997;134:221–229. [PubMed]
- Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A, van Dyck CH, Rosenblatt W, Zea-Ponce Y, Zoghbi SS, Baldwin RM, Charney DS, Hoffer PB, Kung HF, Innis RB. SPECT imaging of striatal dopamine release after amphetamine challenge. Journal of Nuclear Medicine. 1995;36:1182–1190. [PubMed]
- Lett RT. Repeated exposures intensify rather that diminish the rewarding effects of amphetamine, morphine, and cocaine. Psychopharmacology. 1989;98:357–362. [PubMed]
- Leyton M. Conditioned and sensitized responses to stimulant drugs in humans. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2007;31:1601–1613. [PubMed]
- Leyton M. The neurobiology of desire: dopamine and the regulation of mood and motivational states in humans. In: Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC, editors. Pleasures of the brain. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2008. in press.
- Leyton M, Boileau I, Benkelfat C, Diksic M, Baker GB, Dagher A. Amphetamine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine, drug wanting, and novelty seeking: A PET/[11C]raclopride study in healthy men. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:1027–1035. [PubMed]
- Leyton M, Casey KF, Delaney JS, Kolivakis T, Benkelfat C. Cocaine craving, euphoria, and self-administration: A preliminary study of the effect of catecholamine precursor depletion. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2005;119:1619–1627. [PubMed]
- LoLordo VM, Fairless JL. Pavlovian conditioned inhibition: The literature since 1969. In: Miller RR, Spear NE, editors. Information processing in animals: Conditioned Inhibition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1985. pp. 1–49.
- Lorrain DS, Arnold GM, Vezina P. Previous exposure to amphetamine increases incentive to obtain the drug: Long-lasting effects revealed by the progressive ratio schedule. Behavioral Brain Research. 2000;107:9–19. [PubMed]
- Lyness WH, Friedle NM, Moore KE. Destruction of dopaminergic nerve terminals in nucleus accumbens: Effect on d-amphetamine self-administration. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1979;11:553–556. [PubMed]
- Lysle DT, Fowler H. Inhibition as a “slave” process: Deactivation of conditioned inhibition through extinction of conditioned excitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes. 1985;11:71–94. [PubMed]
- Mackintosh NJ. The Psychology of Animal Learning. Academic Press; New York, NY: 1974.
- Martinez D, Narendran R, Foltin RW, Slifstein M, Hwang D-R, Broft A, Huang Y, Cooper TB, Fischman MW, Kleber HD, Laruelle M. Amphetamine-induced dopamine release: markedly blunted in cocaine dependence and predictive of the choice to self-administer cocaine. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:622–629. [PubMed]
- Martinez D, Slifstein M, Broft A, Mawlawi O, Hwang D-R, Huang T, Kegeles L, Zarahn E, Abi-Darghan A, Haber SN, Laruelle M. Imaging human mesolimbic dopamine transmission with PET: II. Amphetamine-induced dopamine release in the functional subdivisions of the striatum. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism. 2003;23:285–230. [PubMed]
- Mattson BJ, Koya E, Simmons DE, Mitchell TB, Berkow A, Crombag HS, Hope BT. Context-specific sensitization of cocaine-induced locomotor activity and associated neuronal ensembles in rat nucleus accumbens. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;27:202–212. [PubMed]
- Mendolson JH, Sholar M, Mello NK, Teoh SK, Sholar JW. Cocaine tolerance: behavioral, cardiovascular, and neuroendocrine function in men. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;18:263–27. [PubMed]
- Mendrek A, Blaha C, Phillips AG. Pre-exposure to amphetamine sensitizes rats to its rewarding properties as measured by a progressive ratio schedule. Psychopharmacology. 1998;135:416–422. [PubMed]
- Mogenson GJ. Limbic-motor integration – with an emphasis on the initiation of exploratory and goal-directed locomotion. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology. 1987;12:117–170.
- Munro CA, McCaul ME, Wong DF, Oswald LM, Zhou Y, Brasic J, Kuwabara H, Anil Kumar A, Alexander M, Ye W, Wand GS. Sex differences in striatal dopamine release in healthy adults. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;59:966–974. [PubMed]
- Nagoshi C, Kumor KM, Muntaner C. Test-retest stability of cardiovascular and subjective responses to intravenous cocaine in humans. British Journal of Addiction. 1992;87:591–599. [PubMed]
- Oswald LM, Wong DF, McCaul M, Zhou Y, Kuwabara H, Choi L, Brasic J, Wand GS. Relationships among ventral striatal dopamine release, cortisol secretion, and subjective responses to amphetamine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:821–832. [PubMed]
- Panlilio LV, Yasar S, Nemeth-Coslett R, Katz JL, Henningfield JE, Solinas M, et al. Human cocaine-seeking behavior and its control by drug-associated stimuli in he laboratory. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:433–443. [PubMed]
- Paulson PE, Camp DM, Robinson TE. Time course of transient behavioural depression and persistent behavioural sensitization in relation to regional brain monoamine concentrations during amphetamine withdrawal in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1991;103:480–92. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Paulson PE, Robinson TE. Amphetamine-induced time-dependent sensitization of dopamine neurotransmission in the dorsal and ventral striatum: a microdialysis study in behaving rats. Synapse. 1995;19:56–65. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Pert A, Post R, Weiss SR. Conditioning as a critical determinant of sensitization induced by psychomotor stimulants. NIDA Research Monographs. 1990;97:208–241. [PubMed]
- Perugini M, Vezina P. Amphetamine administered to the ventral tegmental area sensitizes rats to the locomotor effects of nucleus accumbens amphetamine. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 1994;270:690–696. [PubMed]
- Phillips GD, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Bilateral intra-accumbens self-administration of d-amphetamine: Antagonism with intra-accumbens SCH-23390 and sulpiride. Psychopharmacology. 1994;114:477–485. [PubMed]
- Piazza PV, Deminiere J, Le Moal M, Simon H. Factors that predict individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Science. 1989;245:1511–1513. [PubMed]
- Piazza PV, Maccari S, Deminière JM, Le Moal M, Mormède P, Simon H. Corticosterone levels determine individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. 1991;88:2088–2092. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Pierre PJ, Vezina P. Predisposition to self-administer amphetamine: the contribution of response to novelty and prior exposure to the drug. Psychopharmacology. 1997;129:277–284. [PubMed]
- Pierre PJ, Vezina P. D1 dopamine receptor blockade prevents the facilitation of amphetamine self-administration induced by prior exposure to the drug. Psychopharmacology. 1998;138:159–166. [PubMed]
- Rescorla RA. Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin. 1969;72:77–94.
- Rescorla RA. Conditioned inhibition and facilitation. In: Miller RR, Spear NE, editors. Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1985. pp. 299–326.
- Riccardi P, Li R, Ansari MS, Zald D, Park S, Dawant B, Anderson S, Doop M, Wodward N, Schoenberg E, Schmidt D, Baldwin R, Kessler R. Amphetamine-induced displacement of [18F] fallypride in striatum and extrastriatal regions in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006a;31:1016–1026. [PubMed]
- Robinson TE. Stimulant drugs and stress: Factors influencing individual differences in the susceptibility to sensitization. In: Kalivas PW, Barnes CD, editors. Sensitization in the nervous system. Telford Press; Caldwell, NJ: 1988. pp. 145–173.
- Robinson TE. The neurobiology of amphetamine psychosis: Evidence from studies with an animal model. In: Nakazawa T, editor. Biological Basis of Schizophrenia. Scientific Societies Press; Tokyo, Japan: 1991. pp. 185–201.
- Robinson TE, Becker JB. Behavioral sensitization is accompanied by an enhancement in amphetamine-stimulated dopamine release from striatal tissue in vitro. European Journal of Pharmacology. 1982;85:253–254. [PubMed]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews. 1993;18:247–291. [PubMed]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Addiction. Annual Review of Psychology. 2003;54:25–53. [PubMed]
- Rothman RB, Gorelick DA, Baumann MH, Guo XY, Herning RI, Pickworth WB, Gendron TM, Koeppl B, Thomson LE, Henningfield JE. Lack of evidence for context-dependent cocaine-induced sensitization in humans: preliminary studies. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1994;49:583–588. [PubMed]
- Sax KW, Strakowski SM. Enhanced behavioral response to repeated d-amphetamine and personality traits in humans. Biological Psychiatry. 1998;44:1192–1195. [PubMed]
- Schlaepfer TE, Pearlson GD, Wong DF, Marenco S, Dannals RF. PET study of competition between intravenous cocaine and [11C]raclopride at dopamine receptors in human subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154:1209–1213. [PubMed]
- Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275:1593–1599. [PubMed]
- Scott-Railton J, Arnold G, Vezina P. Appetitive sensitization by amphetamine does not reduce its ability to produce conditioned taste aversion to saccharin. Behavioural Brain Research. 2006;175:305–314. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Segal DS, Kuczenski R. In vivo microdialysis reveals a diminished amphetamine-induced DA response corresponding to behavioral sensitization produced by repeated amphetamine pretreatment. Brain Research. 1992;571:330–337. [PubMed]
- Seiden LS, Sabol KE, Ricaurte GA. Amphetamine: effects on catecholamine systems and behavior. Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology. 1993;32:639–677. [PubMed]
- Shippenberg TS, Heidbreder CA. Sensitization to the conditioned rewarding effects of cocaine: Pharmacological and temporal characteristics. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 1995;273:808–815. [PubMed]
- Siegel S, Hinson RE, Krank MD. Morphine-induced attenuation of morphine tolerance. Science. 1981;212:1533–1534. [PubMed]
- Stewart J. In: Neurobiology of conditioning to drugs of abuse. Kalivas PW, Samson HH, editors. The Neurobiology of Drug and Alcohol Addiction; New York, NY: 1992. pp. 335–346. [PubMed]
- Stewart J. Pathways to relapse: Factors controlling the reinitiation of drug seeking after abstinence. In: Bevins RA, Bardo MT, editors. The Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Motivational Factors in the Etiology of Drug Abuse. University of Nebraska Press; Lincoln, NE: 2004. pp. 197–234. [PubMed]
- Stewart J, deWit H, Eikelboom R. The role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychological Review. 1984;91:251–268. [PubMed]
- Stewart J, Vezina P. Environment-specific enhancement of the hyperactivity induced by systemic or intra-VTA morphine injections in rats pre-exposed to amphetamine. Psychobiology. 1987;15:144–153.
- Stewart J, Vezina P. Conditioning and behavioral sensitization. In: Kalivas PW, Barnes CD, editors. Sensitization in the Nervous System. Telford Press; Caldwell, NJ: 1988. pp. 207–224.
- Stewart J, Vezina P. Extinction procedures abolish conditioned stimulus control but spare sensitized responding to amphetamine. Behavioural Pharmacology. 1991;2:65–71. [PubMed]
- Strakowski SM, Sax KW. Progressive behavioral response to repeated d-amphetamine challenge: further evidence for sensitization in humans. Biological Psychiatry. 1998;44:1171–1177. [PubMed]
- Strakowski SM, Sax KW, Rosenberg HL, DelBello MP, Adler CM. Human response to repeated low-dose d-amphetamine: evidence for behavioral enhancement and tolerance. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25:548–554. [PubMed]
- Strakowski SM, Sax KW, Setters MJ, Keck PE., Jr. Enhanced response to repeated d-amphetamine challenge: evidence for behavioral sensitization in humans. Biological Psychiatry. 1996;40:872–880. [PubMed]
- Suto N, Austin JD, Tanabe L, Kramer M, Wright D, Vezina P. Previous exposure to VTA amphetamine enhances cocaine self-administration in a D1 dopamine receptor dependent manner. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:970–979. [PubMed]
- Suto N, Tanabe LM, Austin JD, Creekmore E, Vezina P. Previous exposure to VTA amphetamine enhances cocaine self-administration in an NMDA, AMPA/kainate and metabotropic glutamate receptor dependent manner. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:629–639. [PubMed]
- Thomas E. Excitatory and inhibitory processes in hypothalamic conditioning. In: Boakes RA, Halliday MS, editors. Inhibition and Learning. Academic Press; New York, NY: 1972. pp. 359–380.
- Valadez A, Schenk S. Persistence of the ability of amphetamine pre-exposure to facilitate acquisition of cocaine self-administration. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1994;47:203–205. [PubMed]
- Vanderschuren LJ, Kalivas PW. Alterations in dopaminergic and glutamatergic transmission in the induction and expression of behavioral sensitization: A critical review of preclinical studies. Psychopharmacology. 2000;151:99–120. [PubMed]
- Vezina P. Amphetamine injected into the ventral tegmental area sensitizes the nucleus accumbens dopaminergic response to systemic amphetamine: An in vivo microdialysis study in the rat. Brain Research. 1993;605:332–337. [PubMed]
- Vezina P. D1 dopamine receptor activation is necessary for the induction of sensitization by amphetamine in the ventral tegmental area. Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16:2411–2420. [PubMed]
- Vezina P. Sensitization of midbrain dopamine neuron reactivity and the self-administration of psychomotor stimulant drugs. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004;27:827–839. [PubMed]
- Vezina P. Sensitization, drug addiction and psychopathology in animals and humans. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2007;31:1553–1555. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Vezina P, Giovino AA, Wise RA, Stewart J. Environment-specific cross-sensitization between the locomotor activating effects of morphine and amphetamine. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1989;32:581–584. [PubMed]
- Vezina P, Lorrain DS, Arnold GM, Austin JD, Suto N. Sensitization of midbrain dopamine neuron reactivity promotes the pursuit of amphetamine. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:4654–4662. [PubMed]
- Vezina P, McGehee DS, Green WN. Exposure to nicotine and sensitization of nicotine-induced behaviors. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2007;31:1625–1638. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Vezina P, Pierre PJ, Lorrain DS. The effect of previous exposure to amphetamine on drug-induced locomotion and self-administration of a low dose of the drug. Psychopharmacology. 1999;147:125–134. [PubMed]
- Vezina P, Stewart J. Conditioning and place-specific sensitization of increases in activity induced by morphine in the VTA. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1984;20:925–934. [PubMed]
- Vezina P, Stewart J. Amphetamine administered to the ventral tegmental area but not to the nucleus accumbens sensitizes rats to systemic morphine: lack of conditioned effects. Brain Research. 1990;516:99–106. [PubMed]
- Vezina P, Suto N. Glutamate and the self-administration of psychomotor-stimulant drugs. In: Herman BH, editor. Glutamate and Addiction. Humana Press; Totowa, NJ: 2003. pp. 183–220.
- Vokow ND, Wang G-J, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress A-R, Jayne M, Ma Y, Wong C. Dopamine increases in striatum do not elicit craving in cocaine abusers unless they are coupled with cocaine cues. NeuroImage. 2008;39:1266–1273. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gerasimov M, Maynard L, Ding Y, Gatley SJ, Gifford A, Francheschi D. Therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate significantly increase extracellular dopamine in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience 21. 2001;RC121:1–5. [PubMed]
- Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gatley SJ, Hitzemann R, Chen AD, Dewey SL, Pappas N. Decreased striatal dopaminergic responsiveness in detoxified cocaine-dependent subjects. Nature. 1997;386:830–833. [PubMed]
- Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gatley SJ, Wong C, Hitzemann R, Pappas NR. Reinforcing effects of psychostimulants in humans are associated with increases in brain dopamine and occupancy of D2 receptors. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 1999;291:409–415. [PubMed]
- Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Logan J, Schyler D, Hitzemann R, Lieberman J, Angrist B, Pappas N, MacGregor R, et al. Imaging endogenous dopamine competition with [11C]raclopride in the human brain. Synapse. 1994;16:255–262. [PubMed]
- Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress A-R, Jayne M, Ma Y, Wong C. Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6583–6588. [PubMed]
- Wachtel SR, de Wit H. Subjective and behavioral effects of repeated d-amphetamine in humans. Behavioral Pharmacology. 1999;10:271–281. [PubMed]
- Wang Y-C, Hsiao S. Amphetamine sensitization: Nonassociative and associative components. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;117:961–969. [PubMed]
- Wolf ME. The role of excitatory amino acids in behavioral sensitization to psychomotor stimulants. Progress in Neurobiology. 1998;54:679–720. [PubMed]
- Wolf ME, White FJ, Hu XT. MK-801 prevents alterations in the mesoaccumbens dopamine system associated with behavioral sensitization to amphetamine. Journal of Neuroscience. 1994;14:1735–1745. [PubMed]
- Wong DF, Kuwabara H, Schretien DJ, Bonson KR, Zhou Y, Nandi A, Brasic JR, Kimes AS, Maris MA, Kumar A, Contoreggi C, Links J, Ernst M, Rousset O, Zukin S, Grace AA, Rohde C, Jasinski DR, Gjedde A, London ED. Increased occupancy of dopamine receptors in human striatum during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;231:2716–2727. [PubMed]