The neurobiology of pathological gambling and drug addiction an overview and new findings (2008)

 

Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008 October 12; 363(1507): 3181–3189.

Published online 2008 July 18. doi:  10.1098/rstb.2008.0100

Abstract

Gambling is a prevalent recreational behaviour. Approximately 5% of adults have been estimated to experience problems with gambling. The most severe form of gambling, pathological gambling (PG), is recognized as a mental health condition. Two alternate non-mutually exclusive conceptualizations of PG have considered it as an obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorder and a ‘behavioural’ addiction. The most appropriate conceptualization of PG has important theoretical and practical implications. Data suggest a closer relationship between PG and substance use disorders than exists between PG and obsessive-compulsive disorder. This paper will review data on the neurobiology of PG, consider its conceptualization as a behavioural addiction, discuss impulsivity as an underlying construct, and present new brain imaging findings investigating the neural correlates of craving states in PG as compared to those in cocaine dependence. Implications for prevention and treatment strategies will be discussed.

Keywords: gambling, addiction, impulsivity, impulse control disorder, brain imaging, functional magnetic resonance imaging

1. Recreational, problem and pathological gambling

Gambling can be defined as placing something of value at risk in the hopes of gaining something of greater value (Potenza 2006). A majority of adults gamble, and most do so without encountering significant problems. Nonetheless, gambling problems among adults have been estimated as high as 5%, with certain groups (young adults, people with mental health disorders and incarcerated individuals) having estimates several fold higher (Shaffer et al. 1999). Pathological gambling (PG), representing the most severe form of problem gambling (see below), has prevalence estimates of approximately 0.5–1% (Petry et al. 2005). Given the increased availability of legalized gambling and its popularity over the past several decades, increased attention to the health impacts of specific levels of gambling behaviours is warranted (Shaffer & Korn 2002).

It was not until 1980 that the Diagnostic and statistical manual (DSM) defined criteria for a gambling disorder (American Psychiatric Association 1980). The term ‘PG’ was selected in favour of other terms (e.g. compulsive gambling) that were arguably more widely used at the time, perhaps in an effort to distinguish the disorder from obsessive-compulsive disorder. Along with pyromania, kleptomania, trichotillomania and intermittent explosive disorder, PG is currently classified as an ‘impulse control disorder (ICD) not elsewhere categorized’ in the DSM. Similarly, in the International Classification of Disorders, the disorder is classified under ‘Habit and impulse disorders’ along with pyromania, kleptomania and trichotillomania. Many of the current diagnostic criteria for PG share features with those for drug dependence (DD). For example, criteria targeting tolerance, withdrawal, repeated unsuccessful attempts to cut back or quit, and interference in major areas of life functioning are contained in the criteria for both PG and DD. Similarities extend to phenomenological, epidemiological, clinical, genetic and other biological domains (Goudriaan et al. 2004; Potenza 2006; Brewer & Potenza 2008), raising questions about whether PG might best be characterized as a ‘behavioural’ addiction.

2. PG as an addiction

If PG represents an addiction, it should share with DD core features. Core components of addictions have been proposed including (i) continued engagement in a behaviour despite adverse consequences, (ii) diminished self-control over engagement in the behaviour, (iii) compulsive engagement in the behaviour, and (iv) an appetitive urge or craving state prior to the engagement in the behaviour (Potenza 2006). Many of these features, as well as others, such as tolerance and withdrawal, appear relevant to PG and DD (Potenza 2006). Concurrent studies of both PG and DD should help define aspects that are related to drugs. That is, drugs may influence brain structure and function in ways that are central or unrelated to the addiction process. In that PG may be conceptualized as an addiction without the drug, direct comparison of both disorders may provide insight into the core neurobiological features of addiction and guide the development and testing of effective treatments.

3. Neurotransmitter systems and PG

Specific neurotransmitters have been hypothesized to relate to different aspects of PG. Based on studies of PG and/or other disorders, noradrenaline has been hypothesized in ICDs to be particularly relevant to aspects of arousal and excitement, serotonin to behavioural initiation and cessation, dopamine to reward and reinforcement, and opioids to pleasure or urges. These and other systems are considered below.

(a) Noradrenaline

Studies performed during the 1980s compared men with PG to those without and found higher levels of noradrenaline or its metabolites in urine, blood or cerebrospinal fluid samples in the former (Roy et al. 1988), and noradrenergic measures correlated with measures of extraversion (Roy et al. 1989). Gambling or related behaviours have been associated with autonomic arousal, with pachinko play and casino blackjack each associated with heart rate elevations and increases in noradrenergic measures (Shinohara et al. 1999; Meyer et al. 2000). During casino blackjack gambling, heart rate and noradrenergic measures become elevated to a greater degree in men with gambling problems as compared to those without (Meyer et al. 2004). In addition to a possible role in arousal or excitement, noradrenaline may be related to other aspects of PG. For example, noradrenergic activity influences prefrontal cortical function and posterior attention networks, and medications (e.g. the noradrenaline transport inhibitor atomoxetine and the alpha-2 adrenergic agonists clonidine and guanfacine) that operate through adrenergic mechanisms have been shown to be efficacious in the treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and other psychiatric disorders (Arnsten 2006). Adrenergic drugs have been shown to influence specific aspects of impulse control in animal and human studies (Chamberlain & Sahakian 2007). These findings suggest several possible roles for adrenergic function in PG and its treatment, and further investigation is needed in this area to examine these possibilities.

(b) Serotonin

Traditionally, serotonin function has been considered to be of substantial importance in mediating impulse control. People with clinically relevant levels of impaired impulse control, including those with PG (Nordin & Eklundh 1999) or impulsive aggression (Linnoila et al. 1983), have demonstrated low levels of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxy indoleacetic acid. Individuals with PG or other disorders or behaviours characterized by impaired impulse control (e.g. impulsive aggression) display different behavioural and biochemical responses to serotonergic drugs than do healthy control subjects. Individuals with PG reported a ‘high’ following administration of meta-chlorophenylpiperazine (m-CPP), a partial serotonin agonist that binds to multiple 5HT1 and 5HT2 receptors with particularly high affinity for the 5HT2c receptor (DeCaria et al. 1998; Pallanti et al. 2006). This response contrasted with that of control subjects and was similar to the high ratings reported previously by antisocial, borderline and alcoholic subjects after receiving the drug. Prolactin response to m-CPP also distinguished the PG and control groups, with greater elevation observed in the former.

Serotonergic probes have been used in conjunction with brain imaging in individuals with impaired impulse control. In individuals with impulsive aggression as compared to those without, a blunted response in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) is seen in response to m-CPP (New et al. 2002) or the indirect agonist fenfluramine (Siever et al. 1999), consistent with the findings in alcoholics (Hommer et al. 1997). Similar studies have not been performed to date in PG, although other investigations have implicated vmPFC function in PG (see below).

Given the data suggesting an important role for serotonin function in PG and impulse dyscontrol, serotonergic drugs have been investigated in the treatment of PG (Brewer et al. 2008). Serotonin reuptake inhibitors show mixed results. In one small, placebo-controlled, double-blind, crossover trial of fluvoxamine, active and placebo arms were significantly distinguished during the second half of the trial, with active drug being superior to placebo (Hollander et al. 2000). A separate small placebo-controlled trial observed no difference between active fluvoxamine and placebo (Blanco et al. 2002). Similarly, one randomized, controlled, double-blind study of paroxetine demonstrated superiority of active drug over placebo (Kim et al. 2002), whereas a larger, multi-centre, randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind study found no significant difference between active drug and placebo (Grant et al. 2003). These initial trials typically excluded individuals with co-occurring psychiatric disorders. A small, open-label trial of escitalopram followed by double-blind discontinuation was performed in individuals with PG and co-occurring anxiety disorders (Grant & Potenza 2006). During the open-label phase, gambling and anxiety measures improved in a largely parallel fashion. Randomization to placebo was associated with a resumption of gambling and anxiety measures, whereas randomization to active drug was associated with sustained responses. Although preliminary, these findings suggest that important individual differences exist among individuals with PG, and that these differences have important implications for treatment response.

(c) Dopamine

Dopamine is implicated in rewarding and reinforcing behaviours and drug addiction (Nestler 2004). However, few studies have investigated directly a role for dopamine in PG. Ambiguous findings have been reported for cerebrospinal fluid measures of dopamine and its metabolites in PG (Bergh et al. 1997; Nordin & Eklundh 1999). Similarly, one early molecular genetic study on PG implicated the TaqA1 allele of the dopamine receptor gene DRD2 similarly across PG, substance abuse and other psychiatric disorders (Comings 1998). Early molecular genetic studies of PG often included methodological limitations such as lack of stratification by race or ethnicity and incomplete diagnostic assessments, and subsequent studies using methods controlling for race/ethnicity and obtaining DSM-IV diagnoses have not observed differences in TaqA1 allelic frequencies in PG (da Silva Lobo et al. 2007). Peer-reviewed publications involving PG subjects and investigating dopamine (or other) systems using ligand-based methodologies do not exist, and such studies represent an important area of future investigation.

PG and other ICDs have been observed in individuals with Parkinson’s disease (PD), a disorder characterized by degeneration of dopamine and other systems (Jellinger 1991; Potenza et al. 2007). Individuals with PD are treated with drugs that promote dopamine function (e.g. levodopa or dopamine agonists, such as pramipexole or ropinirole) or interventions (e.g. deep brain stimulation) that promote neurotransmission through related circuitries (Lang & Obeso 2004). As such, ICDs in PD could potentially emerge from the pathophysiology of the disorder, its treatment, or some combination thereof. Two studies investigated ICDs in several hundred individuals with PD (Voon et al. 2006; Weintraub et al. 2006). ICDs were associated with the class of dopamine agonists rather than specific agents, and individuals with ICDs were younger and had earlier ages at PD onset. Individuals with and without ICDs also differed on other factors related to impaired impulse control. In one study, those with an ICD were more likely to have experienced an ICD prior to PD onset (Weintraub et al. 2006). In another, PD subjects with and without PG were distinguished by measures of impulsivity, novelty seeking and personal or familial alcoholism (Voon et al. 2007). The potential contribution of these and other individual difference variables warrants further consideration in investigations into the pathophysiologies of and treatments for ICDs in PD. Although anecdotal and case series report improvement in ICD symptomatology with discontinuation or diminished dosing of dopamine agonists (Mamikonyan et al. 2008), these studies are preliminary in nature and subject to typical biases of uncontrolled trials. Furthermore, some patients may not tolerate higher doses of levodopa used to control symptoms of PD whereas others might abuse these drugs (Giovannoni et al. 2000; Evans et al. 2005). Together, these findings indicate that more research is needed into the pathophysiologies of and treatments for ICDs in PD.

(d) Opioids

Opioids have been implicated in pleasurable and rewarding processes, and opioid function can influence neurotransmission in the mesolimbic pathway that extends from the ventral tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens or ventral striatum (Spanagel et al. 1992). On the basis of these findings and similarities between PG and addictions, such as alcohol dependence, opioid antagonists have been evaluated in the treatment of PG and other ICDs. Placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized trials have evaluated the efficacies and tolerabilities of naltrexone and nalmefene. High-dose naltrexone (average end of study dose=188 mg d−1; range up to 250 mg d−1) was superior to placebo in the treatment of PG (Kim et al. 2001). Like in alcohol dependence, the medication appeared particularly helpful for individuals with strong gambling urges at treatment onset. However, liver function test abnormalities were observed in over 20% of subjects receiving active drug during the short trial. Nalmefene, an opioid antagonist not associated with liver function impairment, was subsequently evaluated (Grant et al. 2006). Nalmefene was superior to placebo, and liver function test abnormalities were not observed. The dose showing the most efficacy and tolerability was the 25 mg d−1 dose, one that is roughly equivalent to the 50 mg d−1 dose typically used in the treatments of alcohol or opiate dependence. A subsequent analysis of the treatment outcome in PG receiving opioid antagonists identified a family history of alcoholism as most strongly associated with a positive drug response, a finding consistent with the alcoholism literature (Grant et al. 2008). The extent to which other factors associated with treatment response to opioid antagonists in alcoholism (e.g. allelic variants of the gene encoding the μ-opioid receptor; Oslin et al. 2003) extend to the treatment of PG warrants direct investigation.

(e) Glutamate

Glutamate, the most abundant excitatory neurotransmitter, has been implicated in motivational processes and drug addiction (Chambers et al. 2003; Kalivas & Volkow 2005). Based on these data and preliminary findings suggesting a role for glutamatergic therapies in other ICDs (Coric et al. 2007), the glutamatergic modulating agent N-acetyl cysteine was investigated in the treatment of PG (Grant et al. 2007). The study design involved open-label treatment followed by double-blind discontinuation. During the open-label phase, gambling symptomatology improved significantly. Following double-blind discontinuation, improvement was maintained in 83% of responders randomized to active drug as compared to 29% of those randomized to placebo. These preliminary data indicate a need for additional investigations into glutamatergic contributions to PG and glutamatergic therapies for its treatment.

4. Neural systems

Relatively few investigations have examined how brain activities differ in individuals with PG or other ICDs as compared to those without. One initial functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study investigated urge or craving states in men with PG (Potenza et al. 2003b). When viewing gambling tapes and prior to the onset of subjective motivational or emotional response, the pathological gamblers (PGers) as compared to recreational ones showed relatively less blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal change in frontal cortical, basal ganglionic and thalamic brain regions. These between-group differences were not observed during the happy or sad videotape conditions during the comparable epochs of viewing, and the findings are distinct from studies of individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder, who typically show relatively increased activation of these regions during symptom provocation studies (Breiter & Rauch 1996). During the final period of tape viewing, the time at which the most robust gambling stimuli were presented, men with PG as compared to those without were most distinguished by showing relatively diminished BOLD signal change in vmPFC. These findings appear consistent with those from studies of impaired impulse control in other behavioural domains, notably aggression (Siever et al. 1999; New et al. 2002) and decision making (Bechara 2003).

Although other imaging studies have implicated frontal regions in PG (Crockford et al. 2005), multiple investigations have observed differences in vmPFC function in PG. A study of cognitive control using an event-related version of the Stroop colour-word interference task found that men with PG as compared to those without were most distinguished by a relatively diminished BOLD signal change in left vmPFC following the presentation of incongruent stimuli (Potenza et al. 2003a). When performing the same fMRI Stroop paradigm, individuals with bipolar disorder were distinguished most from control subjects in a similar region of vmPFC (Blumberg et al. 2003), suggesting that some elements common to the disorders (e.g. impaired impulse control, poor emotional regulation) share neural substrates across diagnostic boundaries. Analogously, individuals with substance dependence with or without PG showed less activation of vmPFC than did control subjects in a ‘gambling’ task assessing decision making (Tanabe et al. 2007).

In another fMRI study, individuals with PG as compared to those without showed less activation of vmPFC during simulated gambling in contrasts comparing winning and losing conditions, and BOLD signal change in vmPFC correlated inversely with gambling severity among PGers (Reuter et al. 2005). In the same study and using the same contrasts, a similar pattern of diminished activation was observed in PGers in the ventral striatum, a brain region with dopaminergic innervation and which is widely implicated in drug addiction and reward processing (Everitt & Robbins 2005). Based on work in primates (Schultz et al. 2000), studies of reward processing in humans have associated activation of the ventral striatum with anticipation of working for monetary reward and activation of vmPFC with receipt of monetary rewards (Knutson et al. 2003). This circuitry appears particularly relevant to the processing of immediate rewards as the selection of larger delayed reward involves more dorsal cortical networks (McClure et al. 2004). Blackjack gambling as compared to playing blackjack for points is associated with greater corticostriatal activations in PGers (Hollander et al. 2005). However, this study did not include subjects without PG and thus did not investigate how PG subjects differed from those without the disorder. The finding of relatively diminished activation of ventral striatum in PGers in the simulated gambling paradigm (Reuter et al. 2005) is consistent with the findings from studies of reward anticipation in individuals with addictions or seemingly at risk for such disorders. For example, relatively diminished activation of ventral striatum during anticipation of monetary rewards has been reported in individuals with alcohol dependence (Hommer 2004; Wrase et al. 2007) or cocaine dependence (CD; Pearlson et al. 2007) as well as in adolescents as compared to adults (Bjork et al. 2004) and those with a family history of alcoholism as compared to those without (Hommer et al. 2004). Together, these findings suggest that relatively diminished activation of ventral striatum during anticipation phases of reward processing might represent an important intermediary phenotype for substance addiction and ICDs.

5. Appetitive urge states in PG and CD

Appetitive urge or craving states often immediately precede engagement in problematic behaviours such as gambling for PGers or drug use in drug addiction. As such, an understanding of the neural correlates of these states has important clinical implications (Kosten et al. 2006). From a scientific perspective, studies of similar processes, such as craving states in individuals with PG or those with DD, may clarify aspects that are central to the underlying motivational processes across disorders, independent of the effects of acute or chronic drug exposure.

To investigate, we employed data from our published studies of gambling urges in PG (Potenza et al. 2003b) and drug craving in CD (Wexler et al. 2001). As our gambling study involved only male subjects, we restricted analyses to men, yielding a sample including 10 PG subjects and 11 recreational gamblers (CPG subjects) who viewed the gambling, sad and happy videotapes during fMRI, and 9 CD subjects and 6 non-cocaine-using control comparison men (CCD subjects) who viewed the cocaine, sad and happy scenarios, as described previously. We investigated in the following manner the extent to which brain activations in motivational and emotional processing were similar or distinct in a behavioural addiction like PG as compared to the drug addiction CD. We hypothesized that brain regions whose function was influenced by cocaine exposure, such as frontal and anterior cingulate cortex, would be differentially involved in cocaine cravings in CD and gambling urges in PG.

We used a voxel-based randomization procedure to assign statistical significance in the generation of p-maps that identify differences in the manner in which affected subjects’ brain function differs from that of controls across the gambling and cocaine groups during viewing of the addiction, happy and sad videotapes (Wexler et al. 2001; Potenza et al. 2003b). For each subject group viewing each tape type, we generated a t-map comparing the period of scenario viewing as compared to the average pre- and post-tape grey screen baselines. Next, for each tape type, we generated t-maps contrasting the manners in which the affected subjects (e.g. PG) differed from their respective controls (e.g. CPG), generating a PG–CPG contrast. Next, we contrasted the manner in which the affected groups differed from controls across the addictions ((PG–CPG)−(CD–CCD); table 1a, see figure 1A in the electronic supplementary material). At p<0.005 and using a cluster of 25 to increase stringency (Friston et al. 1994), disorder-related differences in the contrasts between affected and non-affected subject groups were observed during viewing of the addiction tapes (table 1a; see figure 1A in the electronic supplementary material) but not the sad or happy scenarios (not shown). The regions of ventral and dorsal anterior cingulate and right inferior parietal lobule were identified during viewing of the addiction scenarios, with relatively decreased activity in the (PG–CPG) contrast as compared to the (CD–CCD) comparison. Within-subject group contributions to these differences are tabulated (table 1a). The anterior cingulate cortex, a brain region implicated in emotional processing and cognitive control in healthy (Bush et al. 2000) and CD subjects (Goldstein et al. 2007), has been shown to activate during cocaine craving (Childress et al. 1999). Cocaine administration activates the anterior cingulate (Febo et al. 2005), and the timing and the pattern of cocaine administration influence anterior cingulate function (Harvey 2004). The difference in inferior parietal lobule activation across subject groups reflects mainly a difference in the neural responses of the control groups to the gambling and cocaine videotapes. The inferior parietal lobule has been implicated in response inhibition components of impulse regulation (Menon et al. 2001; Garavan et al. 2006). Thus, the findings indicate that viewing tapes of different content (e.g. descriptions of a socially sanctioned behaviour (gambling) as compared to an illegal activity (simulated cocaine use)) is associated with differential activation in control subjects of a brain region involved in mediating response inhibition.

Table 1

Brain activations in PG and CD as compared to control subjects.

We next investigated brain regions common to cocaine cravings and gambling urges, hypothesizing that we would identify brain regions that have been similarly implicated in CD and PG, such as diminished activation of the ventral striatum in reward processing in affected as compared to control subjects (Reuter et al. 2005; Pearlson et al. 2007). For each subject group viewing each tape type, we generated a t-map comparing the period of scenario viewing to the average pre- and post-tape baselines. Next, for each tape type, we created t-maps showing activation abnormalities in the patient groups by contrasting each patient group with its respective control, generating PG–CPG and CD–CCD contrasts. Computer-generated comparisons at successive significance thresholds (p<0.005, p<0.01, p<0.02 and p<0.05) were made to identify regions in which the PG–CPG and CD–CCD contrasts demonstrated similar findings. Individual group p-maps were used to identify brain regions contributing to these findings. No brain regions were identified using this procedure for the addiction, happy and sad tapes. As our prior studies demonstrated that the initial period of tape viewing, prior to the reported onset of motivational/emotional response, was associated with significant between-group differences in responses to the addiction videotapes (Wexler et al. 2001; Potenza et al. 2003b), we performed similar analyses focusing on the initial period of tape viewing as compared to the pre-tape baseline. This procedure identified multiple brain regions (table 1b; see figure 1B in the electronic supplementary material) showing similar activity changes in the contrasts between addicted and control subjects during viewing of the respective addiction tapes, and no regions were identified in comparisons involving the sad or happy tapes (not shown).

The brain regions identified as showing common activation patterns in the addicted versus non-addicted subject groups include regions that contribute to emotional and motivational processing, reward evaluation and decision making, response inhibition, and outcome in addiction treatment. In most cases, these regions were activated in control subjects but not in addicted ones. Relatively diminished activation of ventral striatum was observed in the addicted subjects as compared to control subjects, consistent with the findings on tasks involving reward processing in PG and CD subject groups (Reuter et al. 2005; Pearlson et al. 2007). Ventral components of prefrontal cortex, notably the orbitofrontal cortex, have been implicated in the processing of rewards (Schultz et al. 2000; Knutson et al. 2003; McClure et al. 2004), and the lateral region is thought to activate when additional information is needed to guide behavioural actions or when decision making involves the suppression of previously rewarded responses (Elliott et al. 2000). Lateral regions of ventral prefrontal cortex, such as the inferior frontal gyrus, are also considered to be of significant importance in response inhibition and impulse control (Chamberlain & Sahakian 2007). Other brain regions whose activation patterns distinguished addicted and non-addicted subjects in the present study have also been implicated in mediating impulse control. For example, in a Go/NoGo paradigm involving healthy subjects, the insula, precuneus and posterior cingulate were activated during error processing and orbitofrontal cortex and lingual gyrus during response inhibition (Menon et al. 2001). Insular activation also contributes to conscious urges and thus may influence decision-making processes in addiction (Craig 2002; Naqvi et al. 2007). The failure of addicted subjects to activate these regions in the early stages of response to cues that serve as triggers could contribute to poor self-control and subsequent drug use. These findings have implications for treatment outcome for both PG and drug addiction. For example, insula damage has been associated with impaired betting behaviour as evidenced by a failure to adjust bets with respect to odds of winning, and thus impaired activation might be particularly relevant to PG (Clark et al. 2008). Posterior cingulate activation during viewing of cocaine videotapes was associated with treatment outcome in CD subjects, with those who were able to abstain showing greater activation of this brain region (Kosten et al. 2006). Thus, although these results should be considered preliminary given the relatively small samples of each group of subjects, the findings complement the larger literature on PG, drug addiction, impulse control and the neural correlates of treatment outcome for drug addiction. Additional investigations involving larger and more diverse samples are needed to substantiate and extend these findings.

6. Conclusions and future directions

Although significant advances have been made in our understanding of PG over the past decade, substantial gaps remain in our understanding of the disorder. Most biological studies to date have involved small samples of predominantly or exclusively men, raising concerns regarding the generalizability of the findings, particularly to women. Sex differences in gambling behaviours have been reported both with respect to types of gambling problematic for women as compared to men as well as for patterns of development of gambling problems (Potenza et al. 2001). For example, the ‘telescoping’ phenomenon, a process referring to the foreshortened time frame between initiation and problematic levels of behavioural engagement, was first described for alcoholism, more recently for DD, and most recently for problem and PG (Potenza et al. 2001). Given such clinically relevant differences, examinations into the underlying biology of PG should consider potential influences of sex. Similarly, different stages of gambling pathology should be considered in biological investigations, given the data suggesting differential involvements of neurocircuitry (e.g. ventral versus dorsal striatum) as behaviours progress from more novel or impulsive to habitual or compulsive (Everitt & Robbins 2005; Chambers et al. 2007; Belin & Everitt 2008; Brewer & Potenza 2008). Additional considerations include the nature of impulsivity and its relationship to ICDs and substance addictions. That is, it is possible that substance use may lead to more gambling, more gambling may lead to substance use, or that common factors like impulsivity may contribute to excessive engagement in each domain. Clarifying these possibilities in animal and real-life settings represents a clinically and scientifically relevant goal (Dalley et al. 2007). Given that impulsivity is a complex multifaceted construct (Moeller et al. 2001), understanding how specific aspects relate to pathophysiologies of and treatments for PG and drug addictions is important. Finally, PG is arguably the best studied of a group of ICDs that are currently categorized together in diagnostic manuals. Additional research is needed into other ICDs and their neurobiology, prevention and treatment, particularly as these disorders are associated with markers of greater psychopathology and appear currently to go frequently undiagnosed in clinical settings (Grant et al. 2005).

Acknowledgments

Bruce Wexler and Cheryl Lacadie provided assistance with the functional magnetic resonance imaging work presented. Supported in part by: (i) the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01-DA019039, R01-DA020908, P50-DA016556, P50-DA09241, P50DA16556, P50-AA12870) and the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (RL1-AA017539, P50-AA015632), and the National Center for Research Resources (UL1-RR024925); (ii) Women’s Health Research at Yale; (iii) the Office of Research on Women’s Health; and (iv) the US Department of Veterans Affairs VISN1 MIRECC and REAP.

Disclosures. Dr Potenza reports that he has no conflicts of interest over the past 3 years to report as related to the subject of the report. Dr Potenza has received financial support or compensation for the following: Dr Potenza consults for and is an advisor to Boehringer Ingelheim; has consulted for and has financial interests in Somaxon; has received research support from the National Institutes of Health, Veteran’s Administration, Mohegan Sun, and Forest Laboratories, Ortho-McNeil and Oy-Control/Biotie pharmaceuticals; has participated in surveys, mailings or telephone consultations related to drug addiction, ICDs or other health topics; has consulted for law offices and the Federal Public Defender’s Office in issues related to ICDs; has performed grant reviews for the National Institutes of Health and other agencies; has given academic lectures in grand rounds, Continuing Medical Education events and other clinical or scientific venues; has generated books or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts; and provides clinical care in the Connecticut Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services Problem Gambling Services Programme.

Footnotes

One contribution of 17 to a Discussion Meeting Issue ‘The neurobiology of addiction: new vistas’.

Supplementary Material

Figure 1A:

Figure 1B:

Figure legend:

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 1980. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.
  • Arnsten A.F. Fundamentals of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: circuits and pathways. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2006;67(Suppl. 8):7–12. [PubMed]
  • Bechara A. Risky business: emotion, decision-making, and addiction. J. Gambl. Stud. 2003;19:23–51. doi:10.1023/A:1021223113233 [PubMed]
  • Belin D, Everitt B.J. Cocaine seeking habits depend upon dopamine-dependent serial connectivity linking the ventral with the dorsal striatum. Neuron. 2008;57:432–441. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.019 [PubMed]
  • Bergh C, Eklund T, Sodersten P, Nordin C. Altered dopamine function in pathological gambling. Psychol. Med. 1997;27:473–475. doi:10.1017/S0033291796003789 [PubMed]
  • Bjork J.M, Knutson B, Fong G.W, Caggiano D.M, Bennett S.M, Hommer D.W. Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: similarities and differences from young adults. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:1793–1802. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4862-03.2004 [PubMed]
  • Blanco C, Petkova E, Ibanez A, Saiz-Ruiz J. A pilot placebo-controlled study of fluvoxamine for pathological gambling. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry. 2002;14:9–15. [PubMed]
  • Blumberg H.P, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of bipolar disorder: state- and trait-related dysfunction in ventral prefrontal cortices. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2003;60:601–609. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.6.601 [PubMed]
  • Breiter H.C, Rauch S.L. Functional MRI and the study of OCD: from symptom provocation to cognitive-behavioral probes of cortico-striatal systems and the amygdala. Neuroimage. 1996;4:S127–S138. doi:10.1006/nimg.1996.0063 [PubMed]
  • Brewer J.A, Potenza M.N. The neurobiology and genetics of impulse control disorders: relationships to drug addictions. Biochem. Pharmacol. 2008;75:63–75. doi:10.1016/j.bcp.2007.06.043 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Brewer J.A, Grant J.E, Potenza M.N. The treatment of pathological gambling. Addict Disord. Treat. 2008;7:1–14. doi:10.1097/ADT.0b013e31803155c2
  • Bush G.W, Luu P, Posner M.I. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000;4:215–222. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2 [PubMed]
  • Chamberlain S.R, Sahakian B.J. The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry. 2007;20:255–261. [PubMed]
  • Chambers R.A, Taylor J.R, Potenza M.N. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160:1041–1052. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1041 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Chambers R.A, Bickel W.K, Potenza M.N. A scale-free systems theory of motivation and addiction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2007;31:1017–1045. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.005 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Childress A.R, Mozely P.D, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, O’Brien C.P. Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1999;156:11–18. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Clark, L., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Aitken, M. R. F., Sahakian, B. J. & Robbins, T. W. 2008 Differential effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making. Brain131, 1311–1322. (doi:10.1093/brain/awn066) [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Comings D.E. The molecular genetics of pathological gambling. CNS Spectr. 1998;3:20–37.
  • Coric V, Kelmendi B, Pittenger C, Wasylink S, Bloch M.H. Beneficial effects of the antiglutamatergic agent riluzole in a patient diagnosed with trichotillomania. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2007;68:170–171. [PubMed]
  • Craig A.D. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:655–666. doi:10.1038/nrn894 [PubMed]
  • Crockford D.N, Goodyear B, Edwards J, Quickfall J, el-Guabely N. Cue-induced brain activity in pathological gamblers. Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;58:787–795. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.037 [PubMed]
  • Dalley J.W, et al. Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science. 2007;315:1267–1270. doi:10.1126/science.1137073 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • da Silva Lobo D.S, Vallada H.P, Knight J, Martins S.S, Tavares H, Gentil V, Kennedy J.L. Dopamine genes and pathological gambling in discordant sib-pairs. J. Gambl. Stud. 2007;23:421–433. doi:10.1007/s10899-007-9060-x [PubMed]
  • DeCaria C.M, Begaz T, Hollander E. Serotonergic and noradrenergic function in pathological gambling. CNS Spectr. 1998;3:38–47.
  • Elliott R, Dolan R.J, Frith C.D. Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex. 2000;10:308–317. doi:10.1093/cercor/10.3.308 [PubMed]
  • Evans A.H, Lawrence A.D, Potts J, Appel S, Lees A.J. Factors influencing susceptibility to compulsive dopaminergic drug use in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005;65:1570–1574. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000184487.72289.f0 [PubMed]
  • Everitt B, Robbins T.W. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8:1481–1489. doi:10.1038/nn1579 [PubMed]
  • Febo M, Segarra A.C, Nair G, Schmidt K, Duong T.K, Ferris C.F. The neural consequences of repeated cocaine exposure revealed by functional MRI in awake rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:936–943. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300653 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Friston K.J, Worsleym K.J, Frackowiak R.S.J, Mazziotta J.C, Evans A.C. Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1994;1:214–220. doi:10.1002/hbm.460010207
  • Garavan H, Hester R, Murphy K, Fassbender C, Kelly C. Individual differences in the functional anatomy of inhibitory control. Brain Res. 2006;1105:130–142. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.029 [PubMed]
  • Giovannoni G, O’Sullivan J.D, Turner K, Manson A.J, Lees A.J.L. Hedonic homeostatic dysregulation in patients with Parkinson’s disease on dopamine replacement therapies. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 2000;68:423–428. doi:10.1136/jnnp.68.4.423 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Goldstein R.Z, Tomasi D, Rajaram S, Cottone L.A, Zhang L, Maloney T, Telang F, Alia-Klein N, Volkow N.D. Role of the anterior cingulate and medial orbitofrontal cortex in processing drug cues in cocaine addiction. Neuroscience. 2007;144:1153–1159. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.11.024 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Goudriaan A.E, Oosterlaan J, de Beurs E, van den Brink W. Pathological gambling: a comprehensive review of biobehavioral findings. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2004;28:123–141. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.001 [PubMed]
  • Grant J.E, Potenza M.N. Escitalopram treatment of pathological gambling with co-occurring anxiety: an open-label pilot study with double-blind discontinuation. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2006;21:203–209. doi:10.1097/00004850-200607000-00002 [PubMed]
  • Grant J.E, Kim S.W, Potenza M.N, Blanco C, Ibanez A, Stevens L.C, Zaninelli R. Paroxetine treatment of pathological gambling: a multi-center randomized controlled trial. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2003;18:243–249. doi:10.1097/00004850-200307000-00007 [PubMed]
  • Grant J.E, Levine L, Kim D, Potenza M.N. Impulse control disorders in adult psychiatric inpatients. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2005;162:2184–2188. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.11.2184 [PubMed]
  • Grant J.E, Potenza M.N, Hollander E, Cunningham-Williams R.M, Numinen T, Smits G, Kallio A. Multicenter investigation of the opioid antagonist nalmefene in the treatment of pathological gambling. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2006;163:303–312. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.2.303 [PubMed]
  • Grant J.E, Kim S.W, Odlaug B.L. N-acetyl cysteine, a glutamate-modulating agent, in the treatment of pathological gambling: a pilot study. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;62:652–657. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.021 [PubMed]
  • Grant, J. E., Kim, S. W., Hollander, E. & Potenza, M. N. 2008 Predicting response to opiate antagonists and placebo in the treatment of pathological gambling. Psychopharmacology (doi:10.1007/s00213-008-1235-3) [PubMed]
  • Harvey J.A. Cocaine effects on the developing brain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2004;27:751–764. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.006 [PubMed]
  • Hollander E, DeCaria C.M, Finkell J.N, Begaz T, Wong C.M, Cartwright C. A randomized double-blind fluvoxamine/placebo crossover trial in pathological gambling. Biol. Psychiatry. 2000;47:813–817. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00241-9 [PubMed]
  • Hollander E, Pallanti S, Rossi N.B, Sood E, Baker B.R, Buchsbaum M.S. Imaging monetary reward in pathological gamblers. World J. Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;6:113–120. doi:10.1080/15622970510029768 [PubMed]
  • Hommer, D. 2004 Motivation in alcoholism. In Int. Conf. on Applications of Neuroimaging to Alcoholism, New Haven, CT.
  • Hommer D, Andreasen P, Rio D, Williams W, Rettimann U, Monenan R, Zametkin A, Rawlings R, Linnoila M. Effects of m-chlorophenylpiperazine on regional brain glucose utilization: a positron emission tomographic comparison of alcoholic and control subjects. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:2796–2806. [PubMed]
  • Hommer D.W, Bjork J.M, Knutson B, Caggiano D, Fong G, Danube C. Motivation in children of alcoholics. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2004;28:22A. doi:10.1097/00000374-200408002-00412
  • Jellinger K.A. Pathology of Parkinson’s disease: pathology other than the nigrostriatal pathway. Mol. Chem. Neuropathol. 1991;14:153–197. [PubMed]
  • Kalivas P.W, Volkow N.D. The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2005;162:1403–1413. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403 [PubMed]
  • Kim S.W, Grant J.E, Adson D.E, Shin Y.C. Double-blind naltrexone and placebo comparison study in the treatment of pathological gambling. Biol. Psychiatry. 2001;49:914–921. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01079-4 [PubMed]
  • Kim S.W, Grant J.E, Adson D.E, Shin Y.C, Zaninelli R. A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of the efficacy and safety of paroxetine in the treatment of pathological gambling disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2002;63:501–507. [PubMed]
  • Knutson B, Fong G.W, Bennett S.M, Adams C.M, Hommer D. A region of mesial prefrontal cortex tracks monetarily rewarding outcomes: characterization with rapid event-related fMRI. Neuroimage. 2003;18:263–272. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00057-5 [PubMed]
  • Kosten T.R, Scanley B.E, Tucker K.A, Oliveto A, Prince C, Sinha R, Potenza M.N, Skudlarski P, Wexler B.E. Cue-induced brain activity changes and relapse in cocaine dependent patients. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:644–650. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300851 [PubMed]
  • Lang A.E, Obeso J.A. Challenges in Parkinson’s disease: restoration of the nigrostriatal dopamine system is not enough. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:309–316. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00740-9 [PubMed]
  • Linnoila M, Virkunnen M, Scheinen M, Nuutila A, Rimon R, Goodwin F. Low cerebrospinal fluid 5 hydroxy indolacetic acid concentrations differentiates impulsive from non impulsive violent behavior. Life Sci. 1983;33:2609–2614. doi:10.1016/0024-3205(83)90344-2 [PubMed]
  • Mamikonyan E, Siderowf A.D, Duda J.E, Potenza M.N, Horn S, Stern M.B, Weintraub D. Long-term follow-up of impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 2008;23:75–80. doi:10.1002/mds.21770 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • McClure S, Laibson D.I, Loewenstein G, Cohen J.D. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science. 2004;306:503–507. doi:10.1126/science.1100907 [PubMed]
  • Menon V, Adleman N.E, White C.D, Glover G.H, Reiss A.L. Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2001;12:131–143. doi:10.1002/1097-0193(200103)12:3<131::AID-HBM1010>3.0.CO;2-C [PubMed]
  • Meyer G, Hauffa B.P, Schedlowski M, Pawluk C, Stadler M.A, Exton M.S. Casino gambling increases heart rate and salivary cortisol in regular gamblers. Biol. Psychiatry. 2000;48:948–953. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00888-X [PubMed]
  • Meyer G, Schwertfeger J, Exton M.S, Janssen O.E, Knapp W, Stadler M.A, Schedlowski M, Kruger T.H. Neuroendocrine response to casino gambling in problem gamblers. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29:1272–1280. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.03.005 [PubMed]
  • Moeller F.G, Barratt E.S, Dougherty D.M, Schmitz J.M, Swann A.C. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2001;158:1783–1793. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783 [PubMed]
  • Naqvi N.H, Rudrauf D, Damasio H, Bechara A. Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science. 2007;5811:531–534. doi:10.1126/science.1135926 [PubMed]
  • Nestler E.J. Molecular mechanisms of drug addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47:24–32. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.06.031 [PubMed]
  • New A.S, et al. Blunted prefrontal cortical 18-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography response to meta-chlorophenylpiperazine in impulsive aggression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2002;59:621–629. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.59.7.621 [PubMed]
  • Nordin C, Eklundh T. Altered CSF 5-HIAA disposition in pathologic male gamblers. CNS Spectr. 1999;4:25–33. [PubMed]
  • Oslin D.W, Berrettini W, Kranzler H.R, Pettinate H, Gelernter J, Volpicelli J.R, O’Brien C.P. A functional polymorphism of the mu-opioid receptor gene is associated with naltrexone response in alcohol-dependent patients. Neuropsychophamacology. 2003;28:1546–1552. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300219 [PubMed]
  • Pallanti S, Bernardi S, Quercioli L, DeCaria C, Hollander E. Serotonin dysfunction in pathological gamblers: increased prolactin response to oral m-CPP versus placebo. CNS Spectr. 2006;11:955–964. [PubMed]
  • Pearlson, G. D., Shashwath, M., Andre, T., Hylton, J., Potenza, M. N., Worhunsky, P., Andrews, M. & Stevens, M. 2007 Abnormal fMRI activation of reward circuitry in current versus former cocaine abusers. In American College of Neuropsychopharmacology Annual Conference, Boca Raton, FL.
  • Petry N.M, Stinson F.S, Grant B.F. Co-morbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2005;66:564–574. [PubMed]
  • Potenza M.N. Should addictive disorders include non-substance-related conditions? Addiction. 2006;101(Suppl. 1):142–151. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01591.x [PubMed]
  • Potenza M.N, Steinberg M.A, McLaughlin S, Wu R, Rounsaville B.J, O’Malley S.S. Gender-related differences in the characteristics of problem gamblers using a gambling helpline. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2001;158:1500–1505. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.9.1500 [PubMed]
  • Potenza M.N, Leung H.-C, Blumberg H.P, Peterson B.S, Skudlarski P, Lacadie C, Gore J.C. An fMRI Stroop study of ventromedial prefrontal cortical function in pathological gamblers. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003a;160:1990–1994. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1990 [PubMed]
  • Potenza M.N, Steinberg M.A, Skudlarski P, Fulbright R.K, Lacadie C, Wilber M.K, Rounsaville B.J, Gore J.C, Wexler B.E. Gambling urges in pathological gamblers: an fMRI study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2003b;60:828–836. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.828 [PubMed]
  • Potenza M.N, Voon V, Weintraub D. Drug insight: impulse control disorders and dopamine therapies in Parkinson’s disease. Nat. Clin. Pract. Neurosci. 2007;3:664–672. doi:10.1038/ncpneuro0680 [PubMed]
  • Reuter J, Raedler T, Rose M, Hand I, Glascher J, Buchel C. Pathological gambling is linked to reduced activation of the mesolimbic reward system. Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8:147–148. doi:10.1038/nn1378 [PubMed]
  • Roy A, et al. Pathological gambling. A psychobiological study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1988;45:369–373. [PubMed]
  • Roy A, de Jong J, Linnoila M. Extraversion in pathological gamblers: correlates with indexes of noradrenergic function. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1989;46:679–681. [PubMed]
  • Schultz W, Tremblay L, Hollerman J.R. Reward processing in primate orbitofrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Cereb. Cortex. 2000;10:272–284. doi:10.1093/cercor/10.3.272 [PubMed]
  • Shaffer H.J, Korn D.A. Gambling and related mental disorders: a public health analysis. Annu. Rev. Public Health. 2002;23:171–212. doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.23.100901.140532 [PubMed]
  • Shaffer H.J, Hall M.N, Vander Bilt J. Estimating the prevalence of disordered gambling in the United States and Canada: a research synthesis. Am. J. Public Health. 1999;89:1369–1376. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Shinohara K, Yanagisawa A, Kagota Y, Gomi A, Nemoto K, Moriya E, Furusawa E, Furuya K, Tersawa K. Physiological changes in Pachinko players; beta-endorphin, catecholamines, immune system substances and heart rate. Appl. Human Sci. 1999;18:37–42. doi:10.2114/jpa.18.37 [PubMed]
  • Siever L.J, Buchsbaum M.S, New A.S, Spiegel-Cohen J, Wei T, Hazlett E.A, Sevin E, Nunn M, Mitropoulou V. d,l-Fenfluaramine response in impulsive personality disorder assessed with [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;20:413–423. doi:10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00111-0 [PubMed]
  • Spanagel R, Herz A, Shippenberg T.S. Opposing tonically active endogenous opioid systems modulate the mesolimbic dopaminergic pathway. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1992;89:2046–2050. doi:10.1073/pnas.89.6.2046 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Tanabe J, Thompson L, Claus E, Dalwani M, Hutchison K, Banich M.T. Prefrontal cortex activity is reduced in gambling and nongambling substance users during decision-making. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2007;28:1276–1286. doi:10.1002/hbm.20344 [PubMed]
  • Voon V, Hassan K, Zurowski M, de Souza M, Thomsen T, Fox S, Lang A.E, Miyasaki J. Prevalence of repetitive and reward-seeking behaviors in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 2006;67:1254–1257. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000238503.20816.13 [PubMed]
  • Voon V, Thomsen T, Miyasaki J.M, de Souza M, Shafro A, Fox S.H, Duff-Canning S, Lang A.E, Zurowski M. Factors associated with dopaminergic drug-related pathological gambling in Parkinson’s disease. Arch. Neurol. 2007;64:212–216. doi:10.1001/archneur.64.2.212 [PubMed]
  • Weintraub D, Siderow A, Potenza M.N, Goveas J, Morales K, Duda J, Moberg P, Stern M. Dopamine agonist use is associated with impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Arch. Neurol. 2006;63:969–973. doi:10.1001/archneur.63.7.969 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Wexler B.E, Gottschalk C.H, Fulbright R.K, Prohovnik I, Lacadie C.M, Rounsaville B.J, Gore J.C. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of cocaine craving. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2001;158:86–95. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.1.86 [PubMed]
  • Wrase J, et al. Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics. Neuroimage. 2007;35:787–794. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.043 [PubMed]